Glass and McAtee
22 Pages

Glass and McAtee

Course Number: BI 200, Fall 2011

College/University: Montgomery College

Word Count: 14823

Rating:

Document Preview

ARTICLE IN PRESS Social Science & Medicine 62 (2006) 16501671 www.elsevier.com/locate/socscimed Behavioral science at the crossroads in public health: Extending horizons, envisioning the future Thomas A. Glass, Matthew J. McAtee Johns Hopkins Bloomberg School of Public Health, Department of Epidemiology, 615 N. Wolfe Street, Baltimore, MD 21205, USA Available online 29 September 2005 Abstract The social and...

Unformatted Document Excerpt
Coursehero >> Maryland >> Montgomery College >> BI 200

Course Hero has millions of student submitted documents similar to the one
below including study guides, practice problems, reference materials, practice exams, textbook help and tutor support.

Course Hero has millions of student submitted documents similar to the one below including study guides, practice problems, reference materials, practice exams, textbook help and tutor support.

IN ARTICLE PRESS Social Science & Medicine 62 (2006) 16501671 www.elsevier.com/locate/socscimed Behavioral science at the crossroads in public health: Extending horizons, envisioning the future Thomas A. Glass, Matthew J. McAtee Johns Hopkins Bloomberg School of Public Health, Department of Epidemiology, 615 N. Wolfe Street, Baltimore, MD 21205, USA Available online 29 September 2005 Abstract The social and behavioral sciences are at a crossroads in public health. In this paper, we attempt to describe a path toward the further integration of the natural and behavioral sciences with respect to the study of behavior and health. Three innovations are proposed. First, we extend and modify the stream of causation metaphor along two axes: time, and levels of nested systems of social and biological organization. Second, we address the question of whether upstream features of social context are causes of disease, fundamental or otherwise. Finally, we propose the concept of a risk regulator to advance the study of behavior and health in populations. To illustrate the potential of these innovations, we develop a multilevel framework for the study of health behaviors and obesity in social and biological context. r 2005 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. Keywords: Behavior; Causality; Epidemiology-history; Epidemiology-theory; Social class; Social factors Introduction ENOUGH! A century of misunderstanding, the drawn-out Verdun and Somme of Western intellectual history, has run its exhausting course, and the culture wars are an old game turned stale. It is time to call a truce and forge an alliance. Within the broad middle ground between the strong versions of the Standard Social Science Model {caps in the original} and genetic determinism, the social sciences are intrinsically compatible with the natural sciences. The two great branches of learning will benet to the extent that their modes of causal explanation are made consistent. E. O. Wilson (1998, p. 188) Corresponding author. E-mail address: tglass@jhsph.edu (T.A. Glass). 0277-9536/$ - see front matter r 2005 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.socscimed.2005.08.044 The social and behavioral sciences are at a crossroads in public health. Decades of behavioral research has culminated in a series of large-scale intervention trials yielding unsatisfactory results (Susser, 1995). Flagship studies like the (Multiple Risk Factor Intervention Trial (MRFIT), 1982; Stallones, 1983), Community Intervention Trial for Smoking Cessation (COMMIT) (Anonymous, 1995) and, more recently, the Enhancing Recovery in Coronary Heart Disease trial (ENRICHD) (Berkman et al., 2003), have yet to demonstrate the expected efcacy of behavioral interventions to modify health outcomes (Glass, 2000; Relman & Angell, 2002). While it is generally accepted that modest changes in health behavior can be achieved with carefully designed, and theoretically informed interventions, the extent to which behavior change is lasting, or translates into health improvements at a population-level is considerably less clear ARTICLE IN PRESS T.A. Glass, M.J. McAtee / Social Science & Medicine 62 (2006) 16501671 (Glasgow, Klesges, Dzewaltowski, Bull, & Estabrooks, 2004). A consistent criticism of these intervention studies is that they largely ignore the social context that shapes behaviors (McKinlay & Marceau, 2000). A second criticism is that the physiological consequences of behavior change are seldom considered and may be more complex than has been assumed (Sheps, Freedland, Golden, & McMahon, 2003). In sum, these studies illustrate the limits of well-intentioned interventions that treat individual health behaviors as separate from social context and from biological inuences. In the context of etiological studies, important behavioral risk factors for many diseases, especially coronary heart disease, have been identied and well-characterized. Behaviors such as cigarette smoking, inactivity, and a high-fat diet, explain a substantial amount of the worlds experience with atherosclerosis (Beaglehole & Magnus, 2002; Magnus & Beaglehole, 2001). Long-running studies, including MRFIT, the Framingham Heart Study, and the Chicago Heart Association Detection project, have shown that exposure to these traditional risk factors is common to virtually all individuals who develop coronary artery disease (Greenland, Gidding, & Tracy, 2002); although up to 85% of men and women thusly exposed do not develop clinical atherosclerosis. However, despite considerable progress, two problems remain. First, socioeconomic gradients in nearly all health outcomes persist after adjusting for this long list of individual risk-factors (Davey Smith, Shipley, & Rose, 1990; Harding, 2003; Koskinen, 2003; Mackenbach, 1994; Margellos, Silva, & Whitman, 2004; Marmot, Rose, Shipley, & Hamilton, 1978; Marmot, Shipley, & Rose, 1984; van Rossum, Shipley, van de Mheen, Grobbee, & Marmot, 2000; Woodward, Shewry, Smith, & Tunstall Pedoe, 1990). Secondly, the processes that give rise to the social patterning of risks remain poorly described and understood. A great deal is known about the behaviors that lead to disease, but much less is known about how those behaviors arise, become maintained, and more importantly, can be changed. The two problems are obviously connected. Needed is a vigorous and sustained effort to better understand what differentially places people at risk for risks (borrowing language from Link and Phelan, 1995). We need better theory, and better data, to understand how social factors regulate behaviors, or distribute individuals into risk groups, and how those social factors come to be embodied. In this 1651 paper, we offer ideas to advance the study of social determinants of disease, which point toward potentially more effective population interventions. First, we expand, modify, and unpack the stream of causation metaphor. Second, we address issues related to causal reasoning in light of vexing problems created by the study of complex social phenomenon. Finally, we propose a new type of variable, the risk regulator, as a potential solution to several of these vexing problems. Behavior is contingent Behavioral science within public health, especially in the US, has focused primarily on individual health-related behaviors (or life styles), without due consideration of the social context in which health behaviors occur and become socially patterned (For critiques of life style, see Blaxter, 1990; Coreil, Levin, & Jaco, 1985). Obvious examples include exercise, diet, and tobacco and condom use. From Wade Hampton Frost (Maxcy, 1941) to Geoffrey Rose (1985, 1992), public health scholars have pointed out the weaknesses of this approach, arguing for increased emphasis on population determinants, rather than characteristics of individuals. In the last decade, a urry of commentaries in US public health journals has appeared. In this Journal, Krieger challenged us to locate the spider responsible for the web of causation in risk factor epidemiology (Krieger, 1994). Link and Phelan (1995) argued for increased emphasis on what they termed fundamental (social) causes of disease. McKinlay (1995) called for a new public health that treats behavior within a broader social context. Susser and Susser (1996) question the traditional black box whereby disease arises mysteriously from unexamined social forces currently beyond the epidemiologic gaze. McMichael (1999) wondered whether we have become prisoners of the proximate. Ben-Shlomo and Kuh (2002) and Davey Smith (2003) have advanced life-course epidemiology to better understand social gradients in health. Beaglehole and Bonita (2004) noted the isolation of epidemiology from theories and methods from social sciences. In short, consensus has grown for the need to stretch the boundaries of the study of behavior and health in order to capture the role of social structure to a greater degree. Most of these critiques have in common a commitment to an ecological approach (Susser & Susser, 1996) that posits features of the social and ARTICLE IN PRESS 1652 T.A. Glass, M.J. McAtee / Social Science & Medicine 62 (2006) 16501671 built environment above and before the individual (at the familial, community, organizational, and societal levels), constrain, limit, reward, and induce the behavior of individuals. For example, social class is a product of labor relations at higher levels of social organization. Black males in the presence of white power structures are exposed to discrimination (at lower levels of organization). The health behaviors of black males are, in turn, shaped, constrained, and induced from above in a complex relational web.1 This point is by no means new (Macintyre, 1994; Mackenbach, 1998; McMichael, 1999; Susser, 1998; Syme, 1987); however, much of public health continues to treat behaviors such as diet, smoking, violence, drug use, and sex work as if they were voluntary decisions, without regard to social constraints, inducements, or pressures. There are important examples of progress toward contextualizing behaviors (for example research on the role of advertising on smoking initiation), but for the most part, constraining factors on health behavior have been limited to individual psychological factors (such as depression, coping styles, selfefcacy, or readiness to change). Variation in rates of smoking, drug use, or violence across time, space, and between social groups are often ignored, in part because explanation of population-level phenomenon requires different theories, data and methods. The question is: how can behavioral and public health scientists move further beyond this emphasis on individual decision making, toward a more ecological perspective? We address this issue next. Revising a metaphor: extending the stream in three directions Leading theorists in social epidemiology have employed the image of a running stream to describe the chain of causal inuences owing from distal social factors to proximate, individual factors further downstream (Anonymous, 1994; Kaplan, 1995). In this paper, we extend and unpack the metaphor in order to advance the study of behavior and disease in a way that simultaneously accounts for social context and biology, as well as their interactions across the life-course. Our revision (Fig. 1) is based on two primary axes: time and a nested hierarchy of systems from genes, to cells and 1 We thank an anonymous reviewer for the suggestion of this particular example. organs, to social networks and groups, to the global environment. Time is represented by the ow of water across an irregular surface (horizontal axis), while biological and social organization is represented by a vertical axis reecting nested biological and social hierarchies. The vertical axis begins in bedrock (genes), and rises through biological systems lying beneath the surface (underwater), progressing to the plane of the waterline where individual behavior occurs. Above the waters surface is a landscape of ever larger structures that make up the social, built, and natural environments. Together, these elements form a metaphorical landscape of hierarchically arranged spheres of inuence.2 We suggest that the complex interrelationships among these nested levels (and their multiple feedback loops through time) motivate a powerful approach to thinking about social and biological inuences of behavior and disease. A pictorial representation of a three-dimensional space poses graphical challenges. The metaphor acquires depth upon consideration of populations distributed spatially. Individuals are like buoyant objects oating in a network of tributaries, streams and rivers, each beginning a journey at different points and affected by differing topographical features. When envisioned as a three-dimensional space, rather than a plane, the potential richness of the image can be more fully appreciated. Illness states can be represented by pockets in the river bed into which a person might descend. The watershed might contain bumps, hills, or mountains that parallel barriers to adoption of health promoting behaviors. Areas of depression (valleys or canyons) represent opportunities or inducements that attract the ow of water. Depending on differential distribution of resources, some objects oating in the stream may be more able to scale obstacles against the force of gravity, while others may be destined to take the shortest, easiest course. In short, the proposed metaphor offers a rich vocabulary of images for depicting the multi-layered processes that generate population patterns of health. What we have proposed is best understood at the meta-theoretical level. That is, we are describing a general approach to theory construction that addresses issues that cut across a broad range of what Robert Merton might have called theories of 2 A third dimension, representing spatial variation, is implied by the landscape perspective, but not elucidated here. ARTICLE IN PRESS 1653 T.A. Glass, M.J. McAtee / Social Science & Medicine 62 (2006) 16501671 AXIS OF NESTED HEIRARCHIES Uphill Global-level (Geopolitical, economic and environmental dynamics) Macro-level (National/state and large-area dynamics) Mezzo-level (work-sites, schools, communities, healthcare) Micro-level (groups, family, social networks, etc) Constraints Opportunities Down-hill/ Above water Upstream Conception/early-life Human Action/ Behavior Embodiment Underwater Downstream Late-life TIME AXIS Expression Multi-organ system level Cellular level Sub-cellular/molecular level Genomic substrate (the river bed) Fig. 1. The society-behavior-biology nexus as depicted in multidimensional space. The large arrows represent the axes of time and nested hierarchical structures. The sphere of health-related behavior and action moves through time from infancy to old age. Behavior is inuenced by structured contingencies within the social and physical environment and by biological phenomena. Structural contingencies (opportunities and constraints) are shown by paths ending with nodes, while biological phenomena (embodiment and expression) are shown by paths ending with arrows or nodes. the middle range. We are inuenced by the philosopher of science Pepper (1942), who characterized four distinct world hypotheses, which he dened as grand-scale conceptual systems: formism, mechanism, contextualism, and organicism. Pepper argued that each world hypothesis is girded by a root metaphor that functions as a cognitive map guiding scientic discourse. By proposing an alternative root metaphor (the 2-axis stream of causation) we hope to inuence the way smaller scale theories are built and tested. A three-dimensional root-metaphor, like what is proposed here, requires a shift away from simple linear causal thinking that naturally follows from a two-dimensional (upstream-downstream) root metaphor, toward a more complex, more contingent way of thinking. At a higher level of abstraction then, our topographical metaphor also serves to encourage the transition from a mechanistic world metaphor, toward contextualism. In that sense, we are proposing a framework for generating theories and organizing research, rather than a specic theory of how a particular set of explanatory variables can be systematically organized to account for the observed data gathered about a specic outcome of interest. To illustrate how this might be accomplished, an example will be presented later. Ecosocial landscapes: gaining elevation/extending upward One of the limitations of the stream metaphor as used to date, is that the horizontal dimension has typically been used to dene two distinct axes: time ARTICLE IN PRESS 1654 T.A. Glass, M.J. McAtee / Social Science & Medicine 62 (2006) 16501671 and levels organization in social systems. Traditionally, upstream factors are conditions that are temporally and sociospatially distal to disease outcomes. We suggest parsing these two axes. In Fig. 1, the horizontal axis depicts temporal inuences over time, which could be conceptualized at the individual level as the life-course from birth to death, or at the population level in terms of historical changes. Social and environmental forces external to individuals are treated along the vertical axis, as rising above the waterline. Gaining elevation implies that social factors induce and constrain health-related behaviors (and various other proximate risk factors) across the entire life-course, just as a landscape inuences the course of a river. This implies an analogue to gravity; the pull of gravity is analogous to the trajectory of human development and aging. Social structure can be represented as physical obstacles (rocks, boulders, valleys, canyons, and hills) that shape and constrain the ow of water (representing here health-related behaviors). By tracing the topographical features of the social context, additional explanatory power can be brought to bear in understanding patterns of behavior. This does not imply that topographical obstacles are natural features of social context, but rather that human actions and policies manifest themselves as patterns of obstacle placementthe boulders and channels in our metaphorical river. Like the Army Corps of Engineers, policy makers can have dramatic impact on how (and where) rivers ow by altering the placement of obstacles. At the same time, the placement of obstacles in the landscape reects socially constructed power relations, not the hand of mother nature. This threedimensional landscape also creates opportunities for explaining the movement of multiple streams within a watershed. Landscape features closer to the river are lower on the organizational hierarchy (e.g., families, social networks, etc.) compared with higher-level features further uphill (e.g., labor markets, systems of inequity, governmental policies). Uphill forces are distal both in terms of time, but also in the extent to which they have greater leverage in altering the ow of water. Society, behavior, and biology over the life-course: extending horizontally The impact of social context plays out over time and across space throughout the human life-course. This idea is illustrated by recent work in life-course epidemiology (for a recent summary of this literature, see Davey Smith, 2003). Developmental and life-course perspectives in public health are relatively new (Ben-Shlomo & Kuh, 2002; Brunner, 2000; Lynch et al., 1994; Pearlin & Skaff, 1996). At least four important implications of a life-course perspective have emerged. First, early life exposures (both social and physical) explains trajectories of health in adulthood, decades later (Graham, 2002; Hertzman, Power, Matthews, & Manor, 2001; Power & Hertzman, 1997; Van de Mheen, Stronks, & Mackenbach, 1998). Barker and colleagues have stimulated much of this work with their ndings on the fetal origins of cardiovascular disease (Barker, 1991ac). Secondly, the impact of those exposures varies according to its timing. This suggests windows of heightened sensitivity to the effects of certain exposures. For example, studies of work and health show that employment status and working conditions exert their strongest inuence during mid-adulthood (Marmot, Shipley, Brunner, & Hemingway, 2001). Thirdly, the effect of exposure to social conditions appears to be cumulative. Gerontologists have demonstrated increasing heterogeneity over time in health and functional status as a result of differential exposure to social conditions throughout the lifespan (Dannefer, 1988; House et al., 1994; Maddox, 1987). A doseresponse association has been consistently observed between the number and duration of episodes of social and economic disadvantage and increased disease risk (Hallqvist, Lynch, Bartley, Lang, & Blane, 2004; Lynch, Kaplan, & Shema, 1997; Ross & Wu, 1996; Singh-Manoux, Ferrie, Chandola, & Marmot, 2004). Late life appears to be a period of increasing vulnerability to the cumulative inuence of disadvantage across the life course (House et al., 1994; Lantz et al., 2001). Forth, human development at the population level is inuenced by how historically specic events differentially effect the experiences and trajectories of entire birth cohorts. So called cohort effects result in complex patterns as a result of the overlap of early life exposure, differential windows of vulnerability, and cumulative exposure. Moreover, in addition to extending the horizons of our thinking upwards (towards the effect of social context), the proposed model seeks to stretch horizontally, across time, to better understand the cumulative, time-sensitive, historically specic, and duration-dependent effects of social context of human behavior and development. ARTICLE IN PRESS T.A. Glass, M.J. McAtee / Social Science & Medicine 62 (2006) 16501671 Biological embodiment below the surface: extending underwater A central problem in understanding health behavior is explaining how human bodies respond to, and are altered by social conditions over time, and how behaviors are impacted by these biological adaptations. Addiction is an obvious, if somewhat specialized example. As if to anticipate our extended metaphor, Geronimus has coined the term weathering to describe the cumulative changes that occur in bodily systems as a function of repeated exposure to social adversity (Geronimus, 1992, 1996, 2001; Wildsmith, 2002). Krieger and Davey Smith (2004) use the term embodiment, explaining that: ...biologic beings and species are constituted through their engaged interaction with biotic and abiotic environments they in part construct and embody, in the context of dynamic ecologic systems (p 94). Embodiment is an important integrating concept in our model. It describes the sculpting of internal biological systems that occurs as a result of prolonged exposure to particular environments. It is how features of social and built environments become internalized, or get under the skin (Taylor, Repetti, & Seeman, 1997). A long tradition of research has added to our understanding of how external material and social conditions become embodied below that waters surface. Examples include studies of unemployment (Bartley, 1994), acute stress (Benschop et al., 1998; Goldberg et al., 1996), working conditions (Brunner, 1996), social isolation (Cacioppo et al., 2002), social connectedness (Cohen, Kaplan, & Manuck, 1994; Uchino, Cacioppo, & Kiecolt-Glaser, 1996), and chronic stress (Manuck et al., 1987). This work has been strongest in the area of cardiovascular disease where recent studies of allostatic load (McEwen, 2000, 2001; Seeman, McEwen, Rowe, & Singer, 2001) have documented the embodiment of social conditions. Recent advances in genetics has led to concerns about whether interest in the social determinants of population health will be eclipsed by the current enthusiasm for genetic explanations of behavior and health (Allen, 2001). These concerns may be exaggerated to the extent that the potential synergies between genetics and social sciences have yet to be fully appreciated. Understanding the behavioral 1655 determinants of population health is not likely to be improved solely by studying main effects of genes on behavior. Very few complex human behaviors are probably caused by single genes. This is undoubtedly a consequence of the unique developmental trajectory of the human genome toward what Deacon (2000) calls combinatorial complexity; innovation, exibility and variation in human behavior results from the decline of xed genetic determination in highly domesticated species (like humans). The proposed model emphasizes feedback loops and cross-level inuences between physiology (including genetics) and social context. Complex human behavior is the outcome of dynamic exchange among factors above and below the waters surface (to return to our metaphor). Research that explores the interaction between genes and socio-environmental exposures will play an especially powerful role in understanding mechanisms. This work is just beginning (examples include Albeck et al., 1997; Caspi et al., 2003; Epel et al., 2004; Lesch et al., 1996). These examples provide a preview of an emerging multilevel perspective whose characteristics can now begin to be discerned. In summary, we propose a modication and extension of the stream of causation metaphor that species two axes of inuence (time and levels of social and biological organization) and important integrating phenomena (embodiment and structured contingencies, which are discussed below). The purpose of this expanded metaphor is to draw attention to increasing areas of convergence in the study of health behaviors across multiple disciplines in public health and science, which suggests consilience (Gould, 2003; Wilson, 1998) with respect to causal explanations of behavior and health. The model posits a multidimensional landscape of causal inuences that are hypothesized to govern health behavior. At the core of these ideas is a conceptualization of human behavior as sandwiched inextricably between ecology and biology. Diet, smoking, exercise, alcohol consumption, sexual activity, help-seeking, and seat belt use are not free, unconstrained choices. If they were, individualistic, health education interventions would no doubt have proven to be more effective than they have. Health behaviors occur in patterns because they are shaped by social factors residing at levels of organization above the individual, in conjunction with the consequences of ARTICLE IN PRESS 1656 T.A. Glass, M.J. McAtee / Social Science & Medicine 62 (2006) 16501671 biological systems within the body. The important scientic challenges of the future will involve understanding the synergies and dynamics that occur between these nested levels of organization (Institute of Medicine of the National Academy of Sciences, 2001; Shonkoff & Phillips, 2000). This does not imply that behavior is the only route through which upstream/uphill factors effect health. As Marmots work (among others) demonstrates (Marmot et al., 1978; Marmot et al., 1984; Marmot et al., 1991), accounting for health behaviors does not fully explain the association between social conditions and health. However, it is well-recognized that virtually all the major adverse health states that make up the global disease burden, are to some extent rooted in complex human behavior. Thus, health behavior is our topic in this paper, and we believe, a more contextual understanding of smoking, diet, physical activities and health care utilization (among others) would advance the effectiveness of public health policies and interventions. The focus on health behaviors in social context is motivated by Anthony Giddens theories of structuration across time and space. Giddens denes structuration as the active bi-directional process whereby knowledgeable social actors pursue goals within the constraints and opportunities of local environments that are historically and spatially rooted (Giddens, 1979, 1981, 1987, 1993). The aggregated results of these individual actions reproduce (and often alter) the structural arrangements observed at higher levels of organization. While a full explication of Giddens ideas is beyond the scope of this paper, his emphasis on human agency is a useful tool to overcome the vexing micromacro problem (for reviews, see Alexander, Giesen, Munch, & Smelser, 1987; Byrne, 1998). Giddens implies a shift of focus from health behavior (often conceptualized as robotic response to external stimuli), to a focus on health action, as the reciprocal process from which social structure emerges. An emphasis on action places the knowledgeable, strategic, and intention-driven social actor on center stage. It also implies, in our view, the need to prioritize those aspects of social structure that impinge on the consciousness, knowledge base (and decision making) of social actors; a position that accounts in part for our emphasis on behavior and the mid-level social phenomenon that shape those behaviors in the course of purposeful social action. The fundamental causes debate In epidemiology and public health, experimental study designs (chiey the randomized clinical trial) are the gold-standards for evaluating causal hypotheses. Randomized trials allow us to estimate the average causal effect of a treatment by comparing two groups that, because of random assignment, are similar on average measured and unmeasured variables. Differences between these two groups reect the magnitude of the causal association due to treatment. This is because the control group estimates the counterfactual condition (what the outcome would have been in treated subject, if those same subjects had not been treated). However, experimental manipulation of social factors like income inequity, racial discrimination, or neighborhood social disorganization, is not politically, ethically, or practically feasible in most cases. The study of social factors is therefore often restricted to observational data or quasi-experiments. Fundamental thinkers in causal inference, including Holland (1986), make clear that causal models can be tested using observational data, provided that the causal associations hypothesized are not causally meaningless. Holland does not object to hypothesizing causal factors for social phenomenon that are hypothetically subject to experimental manipulation. However, he rules out attributes as possible causes: An attribute cannot be a cause in an experiment, because the notion of potential exposability does not apply to it. The only way for an attribute to change its value is for the unit to change in some way and no longer be the same unit. Statements of causation that involve attributes as causes are always statements of association between the values of an attribute and a response variable across the units in a population. (1986, p 955) Based on this argument, individual attributes like age, social class, race/ethnicity, and gender are not causally meaningful. Kaufman and Cooper (1999) agree, but argue that personal attributes serve as proxies for complex extra-individual social processes that are (in theory) subject to exposability restrictions, and which could be experimentally altered. While it may not be meaningful to talk about the causal role of being Black, the causal effect of racial discrimination as a social process, with specic practices and history, can be imagined. ARTICLE IN PRESS T.A. Glass, M.J. McAtee / Social Science & Medicine 62 (2006) 16501671 In response to this problem, social scientists (House et al., 1990; House et al., 1994; Lieberson, 1985) and public health investigators (Link & Phelan, 1995) have proposed that social factors such as inequity, poverty and racism are fundamental or basic causes of disease. These arguments are not new (e.g., Stallones, 1980; Susser, 1973) and remind us of classic epidemiologic studies of the social production of diseases such as syphilis (Kark, 2003), cholera (Byrne, 1998, Chapter 6; Frost, 1936; Smith, 2002 for a discussion of the classic work of John Snow), pellagra (Morris, 1975), and tuberculosis (Bradbury, 1933). However, the concept of fundamental causes has been met with energetic debate in epidemiology (Krieger, 1994; See Rothman, 1988; Savitz, 1994; Susser, 1998; Vandenbroucke, 1988). Rothman, among others, argues that the real fundamental causes are biological and more proximate to illness, and that the association between social class and disease is a spurious byproduct of the correlation between social class and those real causes (Rothman, 1986, p. 90). The eld has been traumatized by clashes between micro-biologic and the macro-social interpretations of what is fundamental (see for example Shy, 1997; or Vandenbroucke, 1988). These debates have been unhelpful in our view. Competing claims about what is fundamental are ultimately reductionist (in both cases) and block, rather than facilitate new science. Despite the tendency to distill complexity down to false dichotomies (e.g., micro vs. macro, nature vs. nurture, people vs. places), accounting for health behavior requires a more complex idea of causation that includes dynamics occurring across rather than within levels. Next-generation models of health behavior will focus on how the social environment affects (in the probabilistic, not deterministic sense) the organism, which, in turn, affects intra-organismic levels (the organ, the cell, the sub-cellular and the molecular), and how each of these levels feeds back in return to levels above (See Anderson 1998). As has been well articulated before (Macintyre, Maciver, & Sooman, 1993), people create places and places also create people (Bronfenbrenner, 1979). The emerging subeld of neighborhood, or area-based studies, is one illustration of how multilevel thinking can bear methodological and substantive fruit (Diez Roux, 2001; Duncan, Jones, & Moon, 1998; Earls & Carlson, 2001; Elliott et al., 1996; Mayer & Jencks, 1989; OCampo, 2003; Ross & Mirowsky, 2001; Sampson, Morenoff, & Gannon Rowley, 2002; Sampson, 1657 Raudenbush, & Earls, 1997; Taylor, 1997). This literature also illustrates the need for the continued renement of emerging conceptual and methodological tools for thinking across (rather than within) levels. What, then, are the implications of this for the issue of causal reasoning and the problem of meaningless causal associations noted by Holland? Causality, confounding and counterfactuals The goal of observational and intervention studies in public health is to identify causal effects, and to exploit that knowledge in the service of improving population health. This requires a clear set of rules for determining true (as opposed to spurious or confounded) causal effects. While several models of disease causation have risen and fallen in the history of public health research (see for example Greenland & Brumback, 2002), causal reasoning has generally been based, in whole or in part, on a variant of the counterfactual model (or potential outcome model) (Little & Rubin, 2000; Maldonado & Greenland, 2002). According to the counterfactual model of causality, the estimability of causal effects is only feasible and practical when two possible worlds can be imagined: one world that is like ours, and another in which all conditions are identical with the exception of the isolated causal factor under investigation (Greenland & Morgenstern, 2001). The counterfactual model poses potentially insurmountable barriers to identication of uphill (distal) social factors as causes of disease (Kaufman & Cooper, 1999; Kaufman & Kaufman, 2001; Oakes, 2004). In the absence of an experiment, simulating two possible worlds that are identical except for some treatment, it becomes difcult (or impossible) to identify the causal effect of variables like racial discrimination, poverty, or income inequity. It is impossible to imagine a world that is identical to our world, except that there is no (or less) poverty, or that all those in poverty were suddenly lifted into a higher socioeconomic position. That world, were it to exist, would be quite different in a myriad of ways as a result of this change. Therefore, the causal effect of uphill social conditions (or any other causal effect for that matter) are not identiable in observational studies (Robins & Greenland, 1992) and will remain so because it is either unethical or impossible, to design experimental studies of many social factors. ARTICLE IN PRESS 1658 T.A. Glass, M.J. McAtee / Social Science & Medicine 62 (2006) 16501671 The question of whether social conditions can meet the demanding epistemological criteria for causation, operationalized in the counterfactual model, has been met with spirited debate (Rothman, 1986; Rothman, 1988). On one side, traditional views of causation emphasize the Bradford Hill (1965) criteria,3 whereby a risk factor must have strong, specic (if and only if), and unmediated inuences to be classied as a cause of disease (Stehbens, 1985). On the other side, social epidemiologists argue that social conditions can meet causal criteria with minor adjustments (Link, Northridge, Phelan, & Ganz, 1998; Link & Phelan, 1995; Morris, 1975). A shouting match between factions ghting the epidemiology wars has been the result (Poole & Rothman, 1998). Our argument is a practical one. Instead of arguing over what factors qualify as causes of disease, the focus should be on identifying potentially powerful levers4 of behavior change at the population level, regardless of whether those factors are causal in the traditional sense. Thus, we are prepared to concede that social conditions are not disease causes. However, public health might be advanced more rapidly and more efciently, if the focus were on social conditions of life that regulate behavioral risk, but do not strictly qualify as causal because their independent inuences are weak, nonspecic, subject to temporal variation, and contingent on a series of probabilistic, intermediate processes. Following Rose (1992), Rose and Day (1990) and Stallones (1980), it is more efcient to identify population-level factors that inuence rates of disease, and target those for intervention, even if they fail the test of causality at the individual level. So, for example, the cause of death for the victim of a gunshot is the physical damage done by a fastmoving metallic projectile. Had the victim not been shot (counterfactual), she would not have bled to death. However, this statement is independent of whether public health interventions that target the 3 These include (1) strength, (2) consistency, (3) specicity, (4) temporality, (5) biological gradient, (6) plausibility, (7) coherence, (8) experimental evidence, and (9) analogy. 4 A reviewer points out that the allusion to powerful levers of behavioral change implies causation. This is true. However, we mean this in the experimental sense. That is, interventions, such as policy changes, that affect behavior may allow us to demonstrate causal associations between large-scale social conditions and population health in future decades. The example of cigarette taxation and smoking rates in New York City is an example. The capacity of observational studies to identify levers of change at the macro level remains controversial. social conditions that make guns plentiful and violence commonplace, may be more effective in reducing rearm fatalities than issuing Kevlar vests to every person. Having conceded ground in the battle over the denition of disease causes, a partial retraction is required. The purpose of this paper is to nudge the eld toward a more complex view of causality than has persisted during the biomedical regime. The problem posed by social conditions is but one of many challenges that point to the limitations of a Newtonian view of causation. Interestingly, medicine and epidemiology are among the last domains of science that have yet to move beyond the 19th century canon of simple deterministic, linear causal effects. Meteorology, physical chemistry, particle physics, systems ecology, and even economics, have adopted more complex understanding of causality. Epidemiology is being dragged, kicking and screaming, toward complexity and contingent causation. This does not imply a negation of the idea of scientic adjudication, or the possibility of future prediction (as the post-modernists would have it), but rather an acceptance of the inherent complexity of human behavior within a hypercomplex social world. Social conditions, to use the language of systems theory, are control parameters that affect the probability of behaviors that are causes of obesity, lung disease, HIV infection, and violent death. That is not to demote social factors to a lower priority, but to argue that their inuence is second-order, and can only be appreciated (and exploited) in the context of a 21st century concept of disease causation. Given this, we are left with the problem of how to study variables that are clearly related to health, that exist uphill and upstream, but are not causes. What should researchers call social conditions, such as racial discrimination, material deprivation, and cultural norms, if they qualify neither as independent risk factors (causes) or confounders? If social class, gender, and race/ethnicity are markers for exposure to uphill/upstream social and structural processes, how do we classify them for study, in order to better appreciate, measure, and exploit their considerable inuence on health? Their inuence varies over time and place, and their effects are non-specic; they operate probabilistically through a complex chain of intermediate steps that can involve factors at multiple levels or organization. Next, we propose an alternate framework for understanding these kinds of variables. ARTICLE IN PRESS T.A. Glass, M.J. McAtee / Social Science & Medicine 62 (2006) 16501671 Risk regulators and structured contingencies As we have argued, social factors such as racial discrimination, social inequity, poverty, neighborhood deprivation and social capital are difcult to study, in part because variables that measure these social processes do not t the denition of a risk factor. In the absence of an alternative, most researchers treat these variables as potential confounders, and adjust for them in regression models.5 Where do features of social context such as poverty or inequity t? Poverty does not cause any single disease in everyone similarly exposed. Yet, the association between poverty and adverse health behaviors that cause disease can scarcely be ignored. The concept of an independent risk factor, one that causes disease if and only if it is present, and that does so without being contingent on the commingling of other factors, evolved to allow causal inferences within a single level of analysis. How can we begin to construct a multilevel structure for causal inference that is well-suited to explaining health disparities in complex human systems? We propose an alternative class of variable, one that shapes health outcomes in populations, but in a more indirect way. For this purpose, we propose the concept of a risk regulator as a class for variables that capture aspects of social structure that inuence individual action. We dene a risk regulator as a relatively stable feature of a particular patch of the social and built environments, residing at levels of organization above the individual (uphill), but below larger-scale macro-social levels. They are the phenomena that impose constraints and opportunities that shape, channel, motivate and induce behavioral risk factors that cause disease, and the salutary factors that protect against exposure and delay disease progression. Risk regulators index the structured contingencies in the social and built environment as experienced by social actors in discrete action settingsstructured because they are specic, stable dimensions that exist external to individuals, and contingent in the sense that contexts within each dimension are varied, and likely to affect patterns of risk depending on personal, community, and historical processes. They are not themselves risks, but are the conditions that regulate or control exposure probabilities to those 5 The same objections can be raised about the causal role of genes, given that genetic inuences on health rarely meet the Bradford Hill criteria. 1659 distal behaviors (and non-behavioral risks) that lead to disease. Structured contingencies can be described at different levels of organization from families, to local communities, to legal jurisdictions, cultures, societies, and to larger global environments. Risk regulators are, therefore, determinants of disease rates, as opposed to risk factors, which are measures of the specic proximate causes of cases (Schwartz & Diez-Roux, 2001). They function as control parameters that operate at a system level to up- or down-regulate the likelihood of key risk factors (including health behaviors like smoking, inactivity, high-risk sex, and overeating). In contrast to a causal risk factor, a risk regulator operates through multiple pathways and through complex (and potentially non-linear) causal sequences over time and place. Consider an extreme example: war. In 1998, a 6-year war broke out in the Democratic Republic of Congo (DRC), resulting in perhaps the most devastating humanitarian crisis since World War II. In four comprehensive mortality surveys in the DRC, the International Rescue Committee found that the conict has been responsible for approximately 3.8 million deaths in excess of what would have been expected in the absence of war (The International Rescue Committee, 2005). Of the estimated 31,000 excess persons dying in Congo every month, the majority are not caused by war, but are associated with nonviolent causes due to the collapse of the public health and food distribution systems. In all regions of the country and among all ages, violence was responsible for less than 2% of deaths. The vast majority of deaths have been among civilians, and 8090% have been due to treatable and preventable conditions (infection, and malnutrition). In other words, this horrendous society-wide conict is undoubtedly to blame for nearly 4 million deaths, but not because war causes death, but because war has fundamentally altered the social conditions of life in ways that create a new and lethal regime of risk.6 Risk regulators bear resemblance to the strange attractors of chaos theory (Byrne, 1998), or to Link and Phelans (1995) fundamental causes. They differ 6 An informal reviewer pointed out that this example shows that social conditions can meet epistemological criteria for causation. The counterfactual (had the war not happened) is not difcult to consider. The example here illustrates the nonspecicity of social factors. ARTICLE IN PRESS 1660 T.A. Glass, M.J. McAtee / Social Science & Medicine 62 (2006) 16501671 from fundamental causes, however, in several important ways. First, risk regulators are not causes. They are characteristics of social context that help explain the accumulation and distribution of causes. This is more than a semantic distinction. A weakness of the fundamental cause concept is the difculty in explaining changes over time in the precise mechanisms and inuences of, for example, social inequity. Heart disease, obesity, and lung cancer were associated with higher socioeconomic position in the rst half of the 20th century. The direction of that association reversed in the second half as smoking, high-fat diets, and inactivity diffused to lower status groups (Marmot, Kogevinas, & Elston, 1987). This has been used as evidence against the causal role of social class. Secondly, risk regulators are intermediate or mezzo-level variables, midway on the vertical dimension of our causal landscape. They are the bridging tendrils linking larger macro-social processes (such as systems of stratication, labor markets, culture, and systems of production and migration) to the behavioral sphere of human activity and decision making. They reside at levels of organization above the individual, yet below society. In contrast, Link and Phelan dene social conditions in surprisingly individualistic terms as yfactors that involve a persons relationships to other people. (p. 81). Risk regulators are measures of external realities that act to promote or constrain behavior in day-to-day reality. Through the risk regulator concept, we can operationalize how social structure reaches down to affect behavior (which, in turn, becomes embodied and has physiologic consequences). Fig. 2 below is a graphical representation of some key relationships of interest. The gure is not a causal model per se, but a general framework intended to complement Fig. 1 by zooming in on the behavioral sphere. Behavior (or action) is an emergent property of the interplay between a particular set of opportunities and constraints emanating from the environment, and a knowledgeable, goal-seeking actor. The regime of structured opportunities and constraints that exist in a particular patch of space and time, descend from (and are the results of) mediating structures (risk regulators) that lie between larger hierarchically nested systems (depicted in Fig. 1), and the theatre of behavior. Separate from the behavioral realm, these mediating structures act as conduits, through which material exposures and inputs (e.g., air pollution, heavy metals, food, cold temperatures, etc.) challenge the body and must be processed by key regulatory systems. We also show (on the right side) that non-material or symbolic inputs descend from the immediate environment and challenge those same regulatory systems (e.g., threat, support, peer pressure, advertising, social cues for behavior). The social and environmental conditions that give rise to risk regulators are assumed to be hypercomplex. This complexity is alluded to in the gure but not unpacked. The same is true for the biological substrate: including cellular, genetic, and molecular processes of equal complexity. We focus on the bridging structures that shape and delimit individual behavior, recognizing that behavior is only one of the ways through which social context impacts health. Fig. 2 depicts several potentially promising risk regulators including material conditions (Adler et al., 1994; Eachus, Chan, Pearson, Propper, & Davey Smith, 1999; Krieger & Fee, 1994; Macintyre, 1992), discriminatory practices, policies and attitudes (Clark, Anderson, Clark, & Williams, 1999; Krieger & Rowley, 1993; Williams, 1996), neighborhood and community conditions such as housing quality, population density, and fear of crime (Lynch, Smith, Kaplan, & House, 2000; Macintyre, 1994), behavioral norms, rules and expectations (Barabasi & Bonabeau, 2003), conditions of work (Bartley, 1994; Belkic, Landsbergis, Schnall, & Baker, 2004; Bobak, Hertzman, Skodova, & Marmot, 1998; Bobak, Pikhart, Hertzman, Rose, & Marmot, 1998; Bosma et al., 1997; Bosma, Peter, Siegrist, & Marmot, 1998; Hlatky et al., 1995; Karasek et al., 1988; Kuper & Marmot, 2003; Pikhart et al., 2001; Schnall, Landsbergis, & Baker, 1994; Siegrist, 1995; Siegrist, Klein, & Voigt, 1997; Siegrist & Marmot, 2004; Siegrist, Peter, Cremer, & Seidel, 1997), and laws, policies, and regulations (Gostin, 2000; Luepker, 1999; McKinlay & Marceau, 2000; Teret & Wintemute, 1993; Terris, 1980; Wintemute, 1988, 1999). We chose this set of factors because there is an extensive body of literature showing that each is association with health outcomes. Numerous reviews exist that more thoroughly review and explicate the basis for this set of what we are calling risk regulators (see especially Kaplan, Everson, & Lynch, 2000; Lynch et al., 2000; Marmot, 1996; Sampson et al., 2002). An exhaustive listing of potential risk regulators is beyond our scope. What is new is the emphasis on mediating structures (risk regulators), and on health behaviors embedded in social context. What we propose has implications ARTICLE IN PRESS 1661 T.A. Glass, M.J. McAtee / Social Science & Medicine 62 (2006) 16501671 Features of the social, built, and natural environments (see Figure 1 for details): RISK REGULATORS Material Discriminatory Neighborhood/ conditions practices, policies Community (e.g., food and attitudes conditions availability) (e.g., residential (e.g., fear of segregation) crime) Behavioral norms, Conditions Laws, policies & regulations of work rules, and (e.g., cigarette (e.g., expectations taxes) migrant (e.g., dietary labor) practices) CONSTRAINTS OPPORTUNITIES EXPRESSION Material exposures and inputs Cardio-respiratory system Endocrine system Immune system Nervous system Metabolic systems Non-material (symbolic) exposures & inputs Regulatory Systems Genetic and biological substrates (see Figure 1 for details) Fig. 2. Detailed view at closer range of the sphere of human behavior/action from Fig. 1. This detail nested within Fig. 1. Risk regulators, as measures of structured contingencies, produce opportunities and constraints on behavior. They also index the set of material exposures, psychosocial experiences, and information that constitute inputs to which the regulatory systems of the body must respond. Thus, risk regulators inuence behavior indirectly via structured contingencies (opportunities and constraints) and through effects on biological systems inside the body (through embodiment). for the way these associations are modeled and understood. Equally important in the gure (although less well developed here) is the depiction of bridging structures linking the biological substrate to the expression of behavior, and, by extension, larger structures overhead. Of particular interest are the biological systems that process or metabolize both symbolic and material inputs from the external world (including for example sensory stimuli, symbolic messages, and chemical exposures). The systems that regulate heart rate, hunger and feeding, immune response, and sensory inputs are clear examples. These systems negotiate the organisms boundaries with the local physical and social environment. They regulate essential bodily subsystems in response to various inputs, and when necessary, mount countermeasures to rebuff invasion or threat. It is through these biological systems that the limits and characteristics of the human body feed back on behavior, and in turn, on the nature and shape of social structures higher in the systemic hierarchy. Ground-breaking work on the nature of these internal regulatory systems can be found in classical biology including Bernard (1927), whose ideas have recently re-emerged in systems biology (see the excellent discussion by Cziko, 2000). This set of systems (or something similar) perform an equivalent bridging at another role key location on the societybehavior biology nexus. Obesity, an example An example may be useful. In light of surging interest among social epidemiologists, obesity will serve in this capacity. Obesity is a global epidemic with undeniable roots in complex human behavior, ARTICLE IN PRESS 1662 T.A. Glass, M.J. McAtee / Social Science & Medicine 62 (2006) 16501671 with obvious, but as yet unspecied environmental antecedents. Several attempts have been made to explain the obesogenic environment (Ball, Mishra, & Crawford, 2003; Brownell & Horgen, 2004; Drewnowski, 2004; Egger & Swinburn, 1997; French, Story, & Jeffery, 2001; Hill & Peters, 1998; Liu, Cunningham, Downs, Marrero, & Fineberg, 2002; Stunkard, 1977; Swinburn, Egger, & Raza, 1999), although to date, few of these models have been empirically tested with any degree of rigor. Obesity is an ideal example for three reasons. First, individually focused behavioral treatments are costly and, to date, of limited effectiveness (Doak, 2002). Secondly, uphill environmental, social and cultural factors appear to play an important role in shaping the more proximate behavioral patterns that give rise to body weight gain. Studies have shown associations between diet and activity and a range of environmental factors that t into our framework as mediating risk regulators. Examples include community level deprivation (Brunner et al., 1997; Drewnowski, 2003; Drewnowski & Specter, 2004; Robert & Reither, 2004; van Lenthe & Mackenbach, 2002), the spatial distribution of food availability (French, Story, Neumark-Sztainer, Fulkerson, & Hannan, 2001; Morland, Wing, & Diez Roux, 2002; Morland, Wing, Diez Roux, & Poole, 2002), workplace conditions (Rosmond & Bjorntorp, 2000) and food preference and body image norms (Bowman, Gortmaker, Ebbeling, Pereira, & Ludwig, 2004; Dressler, 1983; Green et al., 2003). Thirdly, explaining the obesity epidemic must incorporate the biological substrate. Whatever has changed in the environment that has led to exponential expansion in population body weight, must be conspiring with epigenetic and psychophysiological factors. Eating behavior is an example of a phenomenon that results from synergistic interactions among biological (hunger) and social (eating cues) levels (Cornell, Rodin, & Weingarten, 1989). Human and animal studies showing that environmental stress alters core features of the appetite regulation system, and the metabolic parameters underlying it, offer convincing evidence of this view (Bjorntorp & Rosmond, 2000; Chrousos, 2000; Epel et al., 2004; Spiegel et al., 2004; Tataranni et al., 1996). So, what would a study look like that exemplied our modied stream of causation metaphor, and put risk regulators to work? The arguments above have implications for study design, theory building, and analytic methods. The data needed for our imaginary study would be longitudinal and multilevel. Individual-level and contextual-level data could be collected before, during and after, critical developmental windows during which behavioral patterns are established and body weight changes occur. This would require data at multiple levels including behaviors at water level (physical activity and diet), uphill factors in the environment (food availability, transportation patterns, psychosocial stressors, behavioral norms and other cultural factors) and the biology at work below the surface (basil metabolic rates, biomarkers of pubertal changes, and corticosteroid release). Integrative, multilevel theories would be required to generate testable hypotheses about cross-level interactions and non-linearites. Careful attention to the specication of variable types (risk regulators, risk factors, confounders, mediators, moderators, and outcomes) would be required. The basic anatomy of such a study would include hypotheses about how risk regulators in the social and built environments exert a cascading inuence on body weight through cross-level mediation. Analytic approaches for the study of multiple types of variables can be found in Kraemer, Stice, Kazdin, Offord, and Kupfer (2001). Approaches to modeling multilevel mediation are provided by Raudenbush and Sampson (1999) and by Muthen (1997). What follows reects this previous work. We would hypothesize that ground-level social conditions existing in schools, neighborhoods, and homes (such as cultural norms, area deprivation, laws and policies, and the local food environment) act as risk regulators that inuence two key health behaviors, feeding and physical activity, dynamically and over the life course. Changes in these behaviors and the relative balance of energy intake (feeding) and output (activity) are the primary causes of change in body weight. They are also the primary mediators through which uphill factors (risk regulators) exert contingent effects on body weight. We would further hypothesize cross-level interactions, whereby risk regulators alter biological factors underwater (HPA axis response, mood, appetite, metabolism, gene expression), which in turn directly effect those health behaviors (Fig. 3) Several important feedback loops are hypothesized, which imply that body weight change (embodiment) alters the inuence of biological control parameters. There are important differences between what we propose in Fig. 3 and the existing literature. Our approach is similar to studies that examine, for ARTICLE IN PRESS 1663 T.A. Glass, M.J. McAtee / Social Science & Medicine 62 (2006) 16501671 Hierarchical Axis Effects: Risk Regulators Cultural norms Causal effects: Built environments (e.g., connectivity, walkability) (e.g., food preferences, bodyimage norms) Area deprivation (e.g., poverty, crowding) Psychosocial hazards (e.g., crime, social disorganization) Contingent (indirect) effects: Feedback loops: Local food environment (e.g., availability of fruits/veg, presence of fast food) Commercial messaging Health behaviors Birth weight & early exposures Energy input (feeding behavior) Body weight change Energy expenditure (physical activity) Time Axis HPA axis Mood Metabolism Appetite Genes Fig. 3. . The modied stream of causation metaphor applied to the study of obesity. The contingent effects of risk regulators (i.e., embodiment, opportunity, and constraint) are shown with dotted arrows. Causal effects of biological and behavioral variables are shown with solid arrows. Feedback loops existing within grouped variables are not shown. Specic effects and multiple, time-ordered feedback loops between variables are not shown in order to reduce diagram complexity. example, how dietary behavior and physical activity are inuenced by broader socioeconomic factors (Jeffery, French, Forster, & Spry, 1991; Reijneveld, 1998; Robert & Reither, 2004). In most cases, however, these investigators model environmental factors while adjusting for individual behavioral risk factors in regression models, rather than explicitly modeling more complex causal pathways from risk regulator, to behavior, to body weight (important exceptions include Bobak, Skodova, Pisa, Poledne, & Marmot, 1997; Brunner et al., 1997; Drewnowski & Specter, 2004; Epel et al., 2004). Also different is our emphasis on risk regulators external to individuals. There are many papers that treat individual socioeconomic position as a proxy for differential exposure to risk regulators (Rosmond & Bjorntorp, 2000; Wamala, Wolk, & Orth-Gomer, 1997). Our approach requires that we measure the characteristics of the social and built environments for which individual socioeconomic position is only a poor proxy. Finally, studies that examine the role of biology in modifying behavior-obesity relationships are generally conned to laboratory or animal studies, and do not connect to broader social factors (exceptions include Brunner, 1996; Harburg et al., 1973; Rosmond & Bjorntorp, 2000). The approach outlined here would require careful attention to the role of developmental factors (puberty) and the differential functioning of regulatory systems (appetite, stress response, etc.). Social factors, including culturally embedded dietary practices, commercial messaging, and economic constraints, exacerbate or dampen the inuence of accepted risk factors for obesity. Testing these candidate risk regulators would be analytically challenging. Given the complex, multilevel processes involved, single-equation regression ARTICLE IN PRESS 1664 T.A. Glass, M.J. McAtee / Social Science & Medicine 62 (2006) 16501671 models would be insufcient. Treating risk regulators as confounders in regression is unreasonable and biases estimates of the role of environmental factors. For example, we might be interested in testing the hypothesis that neighborhood disadvantage impacted obesity risk by altering the probability of proximate behavioral risk factors (eating a high-fat diet, physical inactivity). This cannot be addressed through the standard use of interaction terms; the theory does not suggest that the effect of diet is different in those living in neighborhoods high in disadvantage, but rather that poor diets result from conditions of living in those neighborhoods. This problem becomes more complex when considering other individual-level factors such as household income that may limit the types and nutritional values of foods that are available. Given that household income is not independent of neighborhood disadvantage (Sampson & Morenoff, 1997), adjusting for individual socioeconomic status may also lead to biased estimates. Potential analytic methods for assessing the crosslevel effects suggested by this model are available but challenging. Given space constraints, we will limit this discussion to two promising approaches. Harding (2003) has demonstrated the utility of propensity score matching with sensitivity analysis to estimate causal effects of neighborhood-level factors. He argues that this method has advantages over other methods by taking account of the selection of persons into different neighborhood exposures. Disadvantages of his method include the need for binary exposure status (living in a good vs. bad neighborhood) and inability to estimate crosslevel interaction effects (cross-level main effects could be estimated). A second promising approach is the use of instrumental variables. Economists have used instrumental variables to model the impact of policy changes on the probability of persons receiving a given treatment (Heckman & Hotz, 1989; Heckman & Vytlacil, 1999). In many ways, this can be seen as analogous to our idea that risk regulators alter the probability that deleterious (or salutary) behaviors (as in treatments) will be chosen by subjects. These examples highlight how modeling individual trajectories of change in body weight might be approached. However, the ideas here also motivate a shift of focus away from modeling individual trajectories of change, to one that focuses on modeling dynamic systems comprised on interlocking and nested subsystems (for further discussion, see Koopman & Lynch, 1999 or; Prigogine & Stemgers, 1984). Few well-developed examples exist of how to model rates of obesity as system outputs. This will be the subject of a subsequent paper. Conclusion Rose (1985) argued that while distal social conditions are more difcult to observe, they are ultimately more important in determining disease rates in populations because they facilitate the expression of individual susceptibility (such as genetic predispositions, personality characteristics, or individual behaviors). The implication is that the control, and manipulation of these structured contingencies (laws, norms, rules, life conditions) may have greater impact on the publics health than the control of proximate causes. Following Rose, we have argued that the study of behavior and health is at a crossroads. The study of health behavior in isolation from the broader social and environmental context is incomplete, and has contributed to disappointing results from experiments in behavior change. The solution requires a shift in emphasis, a reorientation of theories, and new methods. Evidence for these changes is mounting steadily. Several labels have been proposed including the biosocial model (Eaton, 2001), biopsychosocial model (Anderson & Armstead, 1995; Levi, 1997), ecosocial theory (Krieger, 2001), eco-epidemiology (Susser, 1998; Susser & Susser, 1996), epigenesis theory (Koopman, 1996; Koopman & Weed, 1990), and multilevel science (Anderson, 1998; Diez-Roux, 2000). None of these labels has yet been widely accepted. Debate and cross-disciplinary fertilization is likely to continue for some time before any label nally sticks. We have attempted to contribute to this trend by proposing ideas designed to extend the horizons of behavioral science in public health in three ways: (a) by gaining altitude (to understand causal forces across the topography of social structure), (b) by looking upstream at the interactions of environments and biology across the life course, and (c) by looking below the waters surface at how bodies metabolize (embody) social context. Our model contains some of the elements of similar proposals on which we hope to build, including, for example, Susser (1996), Kaplan et al. (2000) and Lynch et al. (2000). Our approach is different, we believe in several important respects. First, we propose a three-dimensional ARTICLE IN PRESS T.A. Glass, M.J. McAtee / Social Science & Medicine 62 (2006) 16501671 root-metaphor that emphasizes a contextual rather than mechanistic orientation. Second, we address (in a new way) conceptual problems in causal inference as it relates to social phenomenon. Third, we focus on health behavior and the mediating structures that lie between the behavioral sphere and the macro-social context. We propose a novel class of variables to represent those mediating structures. We have been careful to insist that behaviors like eating, smoking, seat-belt use, violence, sex work, exercise, medication compliance, and condom use are not the only avenues through which social factors get under the skin. They may, however, be among the most important. This paper has been an attempt to articulate the broad outlines of what the next-generation approach to the study of behavior and health might look like, without the burden of having to name it. Acknowledgements This was originally written as a concept paper for the ad hoc committee to review the future of social and behavioral sciences at the Johns Hopkins Bloomberg School of Public Health. The authors wish to thank Drs. William Eaton and Scott Zeger, and members of the ad hoc committee, who provided assistance in the development of these ideas. Also, Drs. Brian Schwartz, Brit Creelman, Sander Greenland and Diane Matousek provided productive feedback during the development of this work. This manuscript has also beneted from extensive input from unnamed reviewers. References Adler, N. E., Boyce, T., Chesney, M. A., Cohen, S., Folkman, S., Kahn, R. L., & Syme, S. L. (1994). Socioeconomic status and health: The challenge of the gradient. American Psychologist, 49, 1524. Albeck, D. S., McKittrick, C. R., Blanchard, D. C., Blanchard, R. J., Nikulina, J., McEwen, B. S., et al. (1997). Chronic social stress alters levels of corticotropin-releasing factor and arginine vasopressin mRNA in rat brain. Journal of Neuroscience, 17(12), 48954903. Alexander, J. C., Giesen, B., Munch, R., & Smelser, N. J. (Eds.). (1987). The micro macro link. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press. Allen, G. E. (2001). Essays on science and society. Is a new eugenics afoot? Science, 294(5540), 5961. Anderson, N. B. (1998). Levels of analysis in health science. A framework for integrating sociobehavioral and biomedical research. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 840, 563576. 1665 Anderson, N. B., & Armstead, C. A. (1995). Toward understanding the association of socioeconomic status and health: A new challenge for the biopsychosocial approach. Psychosomatic Medicine, 57, 213225. Anonymous. (1994). Population health looking upstream. The Lancet, 343(8895), 429430. Anonymous. (1995). Community Intervention Trial for Smoking Cessation (COMMIT): I. cohort results from a four-year community intervention. American Journal of Public Health, 85(2), 183192. Ball, K., Mishra, G. D., & Crawford, D. (2003). Social factors and obesity: An investigation of the role of health behaviours. International Journal of Obesity and Related Metabolic Disorders, 27(3), 394403. Barabasi, A. L., & Bonabeau, E. (2003). Scale-free networks. Scientic American, 288(5), 6069. Barker, D. J. (1991a). The foetal and infant origins of inequalities in health in Britain. Journal of Public Health Medicine, 13(2), 6468. Barker, D. J. (1991b). Deprivation in infancy and risk of ischaemic heart disease. The Lancet, 337(8747), 981. Barker, D. J. (1991c). The intrauterine environment and adult cardiovascular disease. CIBA Foundation Symposia, 156, 310. Bartley, M. (1994). Unemployment and ill health: Understanding the relationship. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health, 48(4), 333337. Beaglehole, R., & Bonita, R. (2004). Public health at the crossroads: Achievements and prospects. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. Beaglehole, R., & Magnus, P. (2002). The search for new risk factors for coronary heart disease: Occupational therapy for epidemiologists? International Journal of Epidemiology, 31(6), 11171122 (author reply 11341115). Belkic, K. L., Landsbergis, P. A., Schnall, P. L., & Baker, D. (2004). Is job strain a major source of cardiovascular disease risk? Scandinavian Journal of Work Environment and Health, 30(2), 85128. Benschop, R. J., Geenen, R., Mills, P. J., Naliboff, B. D., KiecoltGlaser, J. K., Herbert, T. B., et al. (1998). Cardiovascular and immune responses to acute psychological stress in young and old women: A meta-analysis. Psychosomatic Medicine, 60(3), 290296. Ben-Shlomo, Y., & Kuh, D. (2002). A life course approach to chronic disease epidemiology: Conceptual models, empirical challenges and interdisciplinary perspectives. International Journal of Epidemiology, 31(2), 285293. Berkman, L. F., Blumenthal, J., Burg, M., Carney, R. M., Catellier, D., Cowan, M. J., et al. (2003). Effects of treating depression and low perceived social support on clinical events after myocardial infarction: The enhancing recovery in coronary heart disease patients (ENRICHD) Randomized Trial. Journal of American Medical Association, 289(23), 31063116. Bernard, C. (1927). An introduction to the study of experimental medicine (H.C. Green, trans.). New York: Macmillan (Originally published in 1865). Bjorntorp, P., & Rosmond, R. (2000). Neuroendocrine abnormalities in visceral obesity. International Journal of Obesity and Related Metabolic Disorders, 24(Suppl. 2), S80S85. Blaxter, M. (1990). Health and lifestyles. London: Routledge. Bobak, M., Hertzman, C., Skodova, Z., & Marmot, M. (1998). Association between psychosocial factors at work and ARTICLE IN PRESS 1666 T.A. Glass, M.J. McAtee / Social Science & Medicine 62 (2006) 16501671 nonfatal myocardial infarction in a population-based case control study in Czech men. Epidemiology, 9(1), 4347. Bobak, M., Pikhart, H., Hertzman, C., Rose, R., & Marmot, M. (1998). Socioeconomic factors, perceived control and selfreported health in Russia. A cross-sectional survey. Social Science & Medicine, 47(2), 269279. Bobak, M., Skodova, Z., Pisa, Z., Poledne, R., & Marmot, M. (1997). Political changes and trends in cardiovascular risk factors in the Czech Republic, 198592. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health, 51(3), 272277. Bosma, H., Marmot, M. G., Hemingway, H., Nicholson, A. C., Brunner, E., & Stansfeld, S. A. (1997). Low job control and risk of coronary heart disease in Whitehall II (prospective cohort) study. British Medical Journal, 314(7080), 558565. Bosma, H., Peter, R., Siegrist, J., & Marmot, M. (1998). Two alternative job stress models and the risk of coronary heart disease. American Journal of Public Health, 88(1), 6874. Bowman, S. A., Gortmaker, S. L., Ebbeling, C. B., Pereira, M. A., & Ludwig, D. S. (2004). Effects of fast-food consumption on energy intake and diet quality among children in a national household survey. Pediatrics, 113(1 Pt 1), 112118. Bradbury, F. C. S. (1933). Causal factors in tuberculosis. London: National Association for the Prevention of Tuberculosis. Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The ecology of human development: Experiments by nature and design. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Brownell, K. D., & Horgen, K. B. (2004). Food ght: The insider story of the food industry, Americas obesity crisis, and what we can do about it. New York, NY: Contemporary Books/ McGraw-Hill. Brunner, E. (1996). The social and biological basis of cardiovascular disease in ofce workers. In D. Blane, E. Brunner, & R. Wilkinson (Eds.), Health and social organization: Towards a health policy for the twenty-rst century (pp. 272299). London: Routledge. Brunner, E. J. (2000). Toward a new social biology. In L. F. Berkman, & I. Kawachi (Eds.), Social epidemiology (pp. 306331). New York: Oxford University Press. Brunner, E. J., Marmot, M. G., Nanchahal, K., Shipley, M. J., Stansfeld, S. A., Juneja, M., et al. (1997). Social inequality in coronary risk: Central obesity and the metabolic syndrome. Evidence from the Whitehall II study. Diabetologia, 40(11), 13411349. Byrne, D. (1998). Complexity theory and the social sciences. London: Routledge. Cacioppo, J. T., Hawkley, L. C., Crawford, L. E., Ernst, J. M., Burleson, M. H., Kowalewski, R. B., et al. (2002). Loneliness and health: Potential mechanisms. Psychosomatic Medicine, 64(3), 407417. Caspi, A., Sugden, K., Moftt, T. E., Taylor, A., Craig, I. W., Harrington, H., et al. (2003). Inuence of life stress on depression: Moderation by a polymorphism in the 5-HTT gene. Science, 301(5631), 386389. Chrousos, G. P. (2000). The role of stress and the hypothalamicpituitaryadrenal axis in the pathogenesis of the metabolic syndrome: Neuro-endocrine and target tissue-related causes. International Journal of Obesity and Related Metabolic Disorders, 24(Suppl. 2), S50S55. Clark, R., Anderson, N. B., Clark, V. R., & Williams, D. R. (1999). Racism as a stressor for African Americans. A biopsychosocial model. American Psychologist, 54(10), 805816. Cohen, S., Kaplan, J. R., & Manuck, S. B. (1994). Social support and coronary heart disease: Underlying psychological and biological mechanisms. In S. A. Shumaker, & S. M. Czajkowski (Eds.), Social support and cardiovascular disease (pp. 195221). New York, NY: Plenum Press. Coreil, J., Levin, J. S., & Jaco, E. G. (1985). Life styleAn emergent concept in the sociomedical sciences. Culture, Medicine & Psychiatry, 9(4), 423437. Cornell, C. E., Rodin, J., & Weingarten, H. (1989). Stimulusinduced eating when satiated. Physiology and Behavior, 45(4), 695704. Cziko, G. (2000). The things we do: Using the lessons of Bernard and Darwin to understand the what, how and why of our behavior. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. Dannefer, D. (1988). Whats in a name? An account of the neglect of variability in the study of aging. In J. E. Birren, & V. L. Bengston (Eds.), Emergent theories of aging (pp. 356384). New York: Springer. Davey Smith, G. (Ed.). (2003). Health inequalities: Lifecourse approaches. Bristol, UK: The Policy Press. Davey Smith, G., Shipley, M. J., & Rose, G. (1990). Magnitude and causes of socioeconomic differentials in mortality: Further evidence from the Whitehall Study. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health, 44(4), 265270. Deacon, T. W. (2000). Evolutionary perspectives on language and brain plasticity. Journal of Communication Disorders, 33(4), 273290 (quiz 290271). Diez-Roux, A. V. (2000). Multilevel analysis in public health research. Annual Review of Public Health, 21, 171192. Diez Roux, A. V. (2001). Investigating neighborhood and area effects on health. American Journal of Public Health, 91(11), 17831789. Doak, C. (2002). Large-scale interventions and programmes addressing nutrition-related chronic diseases and obesity: Examples from 14 countries. Public Health Nutrition, 5(1A), 275277. Dressler, W. W. (1983). Blood pressure, relative weight, and psychosocial resources. Psychosomatic Medicine, 45(6), 527536. Drewnowski, A. (2003). Fat and sugar: An economic analysis. Journal of Nutrition, 133(3), 838S840S. Drewnowski, A. (2004). Obesity and the food environment: Dietary energy density and diet costs. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 27(Suppl. 3), 154162. Drewnowski, A., & Specter, S. E. (2004). Poverty and obesity: The role of energy density and energy costs. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 79(1), 616. Duncan, C., Jones, K., & Moon, G. (1998). Context, composition and heterogeneity: Using multilevel models in health research. Social Science & Medicine, 46(1), 97117. Eachus, J., Chan, P., Pearson, N., Propper, C., & Davey Smith, G. (1999). An additional dimension to health inequalities: Disease severity and socioeconomic position. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health, 53(10), 603611. Earls, F., & Carlson, M. (2001). The social ecology of child health and well-being. Annual Review of Public Health, 22, 143166. Eaton, W. W. (2001). The sociology of mental disorders. Westport: Praeger Publishers. Egger, G., & Swinburn, B. (1997). An ecological approach to the obesity pandemic. British Medical Journal, 315(7106), 477480. ARTICLE IN PRESS T.A. Glass, M.J. McAtee / Social Science & Medicine 62 (2006) 16501671 Elliott, D. S., Wilson, W. J., Huizinga, D., Sampson, R. J., Elliott, A., & Rankin, B. (1996). The effects of neighborhood disadvantage on adolescent development. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency, 33(4), 389426. Epel, E. S., Blackburn, E. H., Lin, J., Dhabhar, F. S., Adler, N. E., Morrow, J. D., et al. (2004). Accelerated telomere shortening in response to life stress. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 101(49), 1731217315. French, S. A., Story, M., & Jeffery, R. W. (2001). Environmental inuences on eating and physical activity. Annual Review of Public Health, 22, 309335. French, S. A., Story, M., Neumark-Sztainer, D., Fulkerson, J. A., & Hannan, P. (2001). Fast food restaurant use among adolescents: Associations with nutrient intake, food choices and behavioral and psychosocial variables. International Journal of Obesity and Related Metabolic Disorders, 25(12), 18231833. Frost, W. H. (1936). Introduction to snow on cholera. New York: Commonwealth Fund. Geronimus, A. T. (1992). The weathering hypothesis and the health of AfricanAmerican women and infants: Evidence and speculations. Ethnicity and Disease, 2(3), 207221. Geronimus, A. T. (1996). Black/white differences in the relationship of maternal age to birthweight: A population-based test of the weathering hypothesis. Social Science & Medicine, 42(4), 589597. Geronimus, A. T. (2001). Understanding and eliminating racial inequalities in womens health in the United States: The role of the weathering conceptual framework. Journal of American Medical Women Association, 56(4), 133136 (149150). Giddens, A. (1979). Central problems in social theory: Action, structure and contradiction in social analysis. Berkeley: University of California Press. Giddens, A. (1987). Time and space in social theory. Social Science, 72(24), 99103. Giddens, A. (1993). New rules of sociological method: A positive critique of interpretative sociologies. Palo Alto, CA: Polity Press. Giddens, A. (1981). A Contemporary Critique of Historical Materialism. Power, Property and the State, Vol. 1. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press. Glasgow, R. E., Klesges, L. M., Dzewaltowski, D. A., Bull, S. S., & Estabrooks, P. (2004). The future of health behavior change research: What is needed to improve translation of research into health promotion practice? Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 27(1), 312. Glass, T. A. (2000). Psychosocial interventions. In L. F. Berkman, & I. Kawachi (Eds.), Social epidemiology (pp. 267305). New York: Oxford University Press. Goldberg, A. D., Becker, L. C., Bonsall, R., Cohen, J. D., Ketterer, M. W., Kaufman, P. G., et al. (1996). Ischemic, hemodynamic, and neurohormonal responses to mental and exercise stress. Experience from the Psychophysiological Investigations of Myocardial Ischemia Study (PIMI). Circulation, 94(10), 24022409. Gostin, L. O. (2000). Public health law in a new century: Part I: Law as a tool to advance the communitys health. Journal of American Medical Association, 283(21), 28372841. Gould, S. J. (2003). The hedgehog, the fox, and the magisters pox: Mending the gap between science and the humanities. New York: Harmony Books. 1667 Graham, H. (2002). Building an inter-disciplinary science of health inequalities: The example of lifecourse research. Social Science & Medicine, 55(11), 20052016. Green, J., Waters, E., Haikerwal, A., ONeill, C., Raman, S., Booth, M. L., et al. (2003). Social, cultural and environmental inuences on child activity and eating in Australian migrant communities. Child Care Health Development, 29(6), 441448. Greenland, P., Gidding, S. S., & Tracy, R. P. (2002). Commentary: Lifelong prevention of atherosclerosis: The critical importance of major risk factor exposures. International Journal of Epidemiology, 31(6), 11291134. Greenland, S., & Brumback, B. (2002). An overview of relations among causal modeling methods. International Journal of Epidemiology, 31(5), 10301037. Greenland, S., & Morgenstern, H. (2001). Confounding in health research. Annual Review of Public Health, 22, 189212. Hallqvist, J., Lynch, J., Bartley, M., Lang, T., & Blane, D. (2004). Can we disentangle life course processes of accumulation, critical period and social mobility? An analysis of disadvantaged socio-economic positions and myocardial infarction in the Stockholm Heart Epidemiology Program. Social Science & Medicine, 58(8), 15551562. Harburg, E., Erfurt, J. C., Chape, C., Hauenstein, L. S., Schull, W. J., & Schork, M. A. (1973). Socioecological stressor areas and blackwhite blood pressure: Detroit. Journal of Chronic Diseases, 26(9), 595611. Harding, D. J. (2003). Counterfactual models of neighborhood effects: The effect of neighborhood poverty on dropping out and teenage pregnancy. American Journal of Sociology, 109(3), 676719. Heckman, J. J., & Hotz, J. (1989). Choosing among alternative non-experimental methods for estimating the impact of social programs: The case for manpower training. Journal of American Statistical Association, 84, 862874. Heckman, J. J., & Vytlacil, E. J. (1999). Local instrumental variables and latent variable models for identifying and bounding treatment effects. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 96(8), 47304734. Hertzman, C., Power, C., Matthews, S., & Manor, O. (2001). Using an interactive framework of society and lifecourse to explain self-rated health in early adulthood. Social Science & Medicine, 53(12), 15751585. Hill, A. B. (1965). The Environment and disease: Association or causation? Proceedings of the Royal Society of Medicine, 58, 295300. Hill, J. O., & Peters, J. C. (1998). Environmental contributions to the obesity epidemic. Science, 280(5368), 13711374. Hlatky, M. A., Lam, L. C., Lee, K. L., Clapp-Channing, N. E., Williams, R. B., Pryor, D. B., et al. (1995). Job strain and the prevalence and outcome of coronary artery disease. Circulation, 92(3), 327333. Holland, P. W. (1986). Statistics and causal inference. Journal of American Statistical Association, 81, 945960. House, J. S., Kessler, R. C., Herzog, A. R., Mero, R. P., Kinney, A. M., & Breslow, M. J. (1990). Age, socioeconomic status and health. The Milbank Quarterly, 86(3), 383411. House, J. S., Lepkowski, J. M., Kinney, A. M., Mero, R. P., Kessler, R. C., & Herzog, A. R. (1994). The social stratication of aging and health. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 35(3), 213234. Institute of Medicine of the National Academy of Sciences. (2001). Health and behavior: The interplay of biological, ARTICLE IN PRESS 1668 T.A. Glass, M.J. McAtee / Social Science & Medicine 62 (2006) 16501671 behavioral, and societal inuences committee on health and behavior: Research, practice, and policy. board on neuroscience and behavioral health. Washington, DC: National Academy Press. Jeffery, R. W., French, S. A., Forster, J. L., & Spry, V. M. (1991). Socioeconomic status differences in health behaviors related to obesity: The healthy worker project. International Journal of Obesity, 15(10), 689696. Kaplan, G. A. (1995). Where do shared pathways lead? Some reections on a research agenda. Psychosomatic Medicine, 57(3), 208212. Kaplan, G. A., Everson, S. A., & Lynch, J. W. (2000). The contribution of social and behavioral research to an understanding of the distribution of disease: A multilevel approach. In B. D. Smedley, & S. L. Syme (Eds.), Promoting health: Intervention strategies from social and behavioral research (pp. 3780). Washington, DC: National Academies Press. Karasek, R. A., Theorell, T., Schwartz, J. E., Schnall, P. L., Pieper, C. F., & Michela, J. L. (1988). Job characteristics in relation to the prevalence of myocardial infarction in the US Health Examination Survey (HES) and the Health and Nutrition Examination Survey (HANES). American Journal of Public Health, 78(8), 910918. Kark, S. L. (2003). The social pathology of syphilis in Africans. 1949. International Journal of Epidemiology, 32(2), 181186. Kaufman, J. S., & Cooper, R. S. (1999). Seeking causal explanations in social epidemiology. American Journal of Epidemiology, 150(2), 113120. Kaufman, J. S., & Kaufman, S. (2001). Assessment of structured socioeconomic effects on health. Epidemiology, 12(2), 157167. Koopman, J. S. (1996). Emerging objectives and methods in epidemiology. American Journal of Public Health, 86(5), 630632. Koopman, J. S., & Lynch, J. W. (1999). Individual causal models and population system models in epidemiology. American Journal of Public Health, 89(8), 11701174. Koopman, J. S., & Weed, D. L. (1990). Epigenesis theory: A mathematical model relating causal concepts of pathogenesis in individuals to disease patterns in populations. American Journal of Epidemiology, 132(2), 366390. Koskinen, S. (2003). Commentary: Is there a common background behind growing inequalities in mortality in Western European countries? International Journal of Epidemiology, 32(5), 838839. Kraemer, H. C., Stice, E., Kazdin, A., Offord, D., & Kupfer, D. (2001). How do risk factors work together? Mediators, moderators, and independent, overlapping, and proxy risk factors. American Journal of Psychiatry, 158(6), 848856. Krieger, N. (1994). Epidemiology and the web of causation: Has anyone seen the spider? Social Science & Medicine, 887903. Krieger, N. (2001). Theories for social epidemiology in the 21st century: An ecosocial perspective. International Journal of Epidemiology, 30(4), 668677. Krieger, N., & Davey Smith, G. (2004). Bodies count, and body counts: Social epidemiology and embodying inequality. Epidemiologic Reviews, 26, 92103. Krieger, N., & Fee, E. (1994). Social class: The missing link in US health data. International Journal of Health Services, 24(1), 2544. Krieger, N., & Rowley (1993). Racism, sexism, and social class: Implications for studies of health, disease, and wellbeing. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 9(Suppl.), 82122. Kuper, H., & Marmot, M. (2003). Job strain, job demands, decision latitude, and risk of coronary heart disease within the Whitehall II study. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health, 57(2), 147153. Lantz, P. M., Lynch, J. W., House, J. S., Lepkowski, J. M., Mero, R. P., Musick, M. A., et al. (2001). Socioeconomic disparities in health change in a longitudinal study of US adults: The role of health-risk behaviors. Social Science & Medicine, 53(1), 2940. Lesch, K. P., Bengel, D., Heils, A., Sabol, S. Z., Greenberg, B. D., Petri, S., et al. (1996). Association of anxiety-related traits with a polymorphism in the serotonin transporter gene regulatory region. Science, 274(5292), 15271531. Levi, L. (1997). A biopsychosocial approach to etiology and pathogenesis. Acta Physiologica Scandinavica Supplementum, 640, 103106. Lieberson, S. (1985). Making it count: The improvement of social research and theory. Berkeley: University of California Press. Link, B. G., Northridge, M. E., Phelan, J. C., & Ganz, M. L. (1998). Social epidemiology and the fundamental cause concept: On the structuring of effective cancer screens by socioeconomic status. Milbank Quarterly, 76(3), 375402. Link, B. G., & Phelan, J. (1995). Social conditions as fundamental causes of disease. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, Extra Issue, 8094. Little, R. J., & Rubin, D. B. (2000). Causal effects in clinical and epidemiological studies via potential outcomes: Concepts and analytical approaches. Annual Review of Public Health, 21, 121145. Liu, G. C., Cunningham, C., Downs, S. M., Marrero, D. G., & Fineberg, N. (2002). A spatial analysis of obesogenic environments for children. Proceedings of the AMIA Symposium, 459463. Luepker, R. V. (1999). How physically active are American children and what can we do about it? International Journal of Obesity and Related Metabolic Disorders, 23(Suppl. 2), S12S17. Lynch, J. W., Kaplan, G. A., & Shema, S. J. (1997). Cumulative impact of sustained economic hardship on physical, cognitive, psychological, and social functioning. New England Journal of Medicne, 337(26), 18891895. Lynch, J. W., Kaplan, G. A., Cohen, R. D., Kauhanen, J., Wilson, T. W., Smith, N. L., et al. (1994). Childhood and adult socioeconomic status as predictors of mortality in Finland. The Lancet, 343(8896), 524527. Lynch, J. W., Smith, G. D., Kaplan, G. A., & House, J. S. (2000). Income inequality and mortality: Importance to health of individual income, psychosocial environment, or material conditions. British Medical Journal, 320(7243), 12001204. Macintyre, S. (1992). The effects of family position and status on health. Social Science & Medicine, 35(4), 453464. Macintyre, S. (1994). Understanding the social patterning of health: The role of the social sciences. Journal of Public Health Medicine, 16(1), 5359. Macintyre, S., Maciver, S., & Sooman, A. (1993). Area, class and health: Should we be focusing on places or people? Journal of Social Policy, 22(2), 213234. ARTICLE IN PRESS T.A. Glass, M.J. McAtee / Social Science & Medicine 62 (2006) 16501671 Mackenbach, J. P. (1994). Socioeconomic inequalities in health in The Netherlands: Impact of a ve year research programme. British Medical Journal, 309(6967), 14871491. Mackenbach, J. P. (1998). Multilevel ecoepidemiology and parsimony. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health, 52(10), 614615. Maddox, G. L. (1987). Aging differently. The Gerontologist, 27, 557564. Magnus, P., & Beaglehole, R. (2001). The real contribution of the major risk factors to the coronary epidemics: Time to end the only 50% myth. Archives of Internal Medicine, 161(22), 26572660. Maldonado, G., & Greenland, S. (2002). Estimating causal effects. International Journal of Epidemiology, 31(2), 422429. Manuck, S. B., Henry, J. P., Anderson, D. E., Clarkson, T. B., Folkow, B., Kaplan, J. R., et al. (1987). Biobehavioral mechanisms in coronary artery disease. Chronic stress. Circulation, 76(1 Pt 2), I158I163. Margellos, H., Silva, A., & Whitman, S. (2004). Comparison of health status indicators in Chicago: Are blackwhite disparities worsening? American Journal of Public Health, 94(1), 116121. Marmot, M. (1996). The social pattern of health and disease. In D. Blane, E. Brunner, & R. Wilkinson (Eds.), Health and social organization: Towards a health policy for the twenty-rst century (pp. 4267). London: Routledge. Marmot, M., Shipley, M., Brunner, E., & Hemingway, H. (2001). Relative contribution of early life and adult socioeconomic factors to adult morbidity in the Whitehall II study. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health, 55(5), 301307. Marmot, M. G., Kogevinas, M., & Elston, M. A. (1987). Social/ economic status and disease. Annual Review of Public Health, 8, 111135. Marmot, M. G., Rose, G., Shipley, M., & Hamilton, P. J. (1978). Employment grade and coronary heart disease in British civil servants. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health, 32(4), 244249. Marmot, M. G., Shipley, M. J., & Rose, G. (1984). Inequalities in death-specic explanations of a general pattern? The Lancet, 1(8384), 10031006. Marmot, M. G., Smith, G. D., Stansfeld, S., Patel, C., North, F., Head, J., et al. (1991). Health inequalities among British civil servants: The Whitehall II study. The Lancet, 337(8754), 13871393. Maxcy, K. F. (1941). Papers of wade Hampton frost. New York: Commonwealth Fund. Mayer, S. E., & Jencks, C. (1989). Growing up in poor neighborhoods: How much does it matter? Science Neuropsychopharmacology, 243, 14411445. McEwen, B. S. (2000). Allostasis and allostatic load: Implications for neuropsychopharmacology. Neuropsychopharmacology, 22(2), 108124. McEwen, B. S. (2001). From molecules to mind. Stress, individual differences, and the social environment. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 935, 4249. McKinlay, J. B. (1995). The new public health approach to improving physical activity and autonomy in older populations. In E. Heikkinen, J. Kuusinen, & I. Ruoppila (Eds.), Preparation for aging (pp. 87103). New York: Plenum Press. McKinlay, J. B., & Marceau, L. D. (2000). Upstream healthy public policy: Lessons from the battle of tobacco. International Journal of Health Services, 30(1), 4969. 1669 McMichael, A. J. (1999). Prisoners of the proximate: Loosening the constraints on epidemiology in an age of change. American Journal of Epidemiology, 149(10), 887897. Morland, K., Wing, S., & Diez Roux, A. (2002). The contextual effect of the local food environment on residents diets: The atherosclerosis risk in communities study. American Journal of Public Health, 92(11), 17611767. Morland, K., Wing, S., Diez Roux, A., & Poole, C. (2002). Neighborhood characteristics associated with the location of food stores and food service places. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 22(1), 2329. Morris, J. N. (1975). Uses of epidemiology. New York: Churchhill Livingstone. Multiple risk factor intervention trial (MRFIT). (1982). Risk factor changes and mortality results. Multiple Risk Factor Intervention Trial Research Group. Journal of American Medial Association, 248(12), 14651477. Muthen, B. (1997). Latent variable modeling of longitudinal and multilevel data. Sociological Methodology, 27, 453480. OCampo, P. (2003). Invited commentary: Advancing theory and methods for multilevel models of residential neighborhoods and health. American Journal of Epidemiology, 157(1), 913. Oakes, J. M. (2004). Causal inference and the relevance of social epidemiology. Social Science & Medicine, 58(10), 19691971. Pearlin, L. I., & Skaff, M. M. (1996). Stress and the life course: A paradigmatic alliance. Gerontologist, 36(2), 239247. Pepper, S. C. (1942). World hypotheses: A study in evidence. University of California Press. Pikhart, H., Bobak, M., Siegrist, J., Pajak, A., Rywik, S., Kyshegyi, J., et al. (2001). Psychosocial work characteristics and self rated health in four post-communist countries. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health, 55(9), 624630. Poole, C., & Rothman, K. J. (1998). Our conscientious objection to the epidemiology wars. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health, 52(10), 613614. Power, C., & Hertzman, C. (1997). Social and biological pathways linking early life and adult disease. British Medical Bulletin, 53(1), 210221. Prigogine, I., & Stemgers, I. (1984). Order out of chaos. New York: Bantam. Raudenbush, S. W., & Sampson, R. (1999). Assessing direct and indirect effects in multilevel designs with latent variables. Sociological Methods and Research, 28(2), 123153. Reijneveld, S. A. (1998). The impact of individual and area characteristics on urban socioeconomic differences in health and smoking. International Journal of Epidemiology, 27(1), 3340. Relman, A. S., & Angell, M. (2002). Resolved: Psychosocial interventions can improve clinical outcomes in organic disease (Con). Psychosomatic Medicine, 64(4), 558563. Robert, S. A., & Reither, E. N. (2004). A multilevel analysis of race, community disadvantage, and body mass index among adults in the US. Social Science & Medicine, 59(12), 24212434. Robins, J. M., & Greenland, S. (1992). Identiability and exchangeability for direct and indirect effects. Epidemiology, 3(2), 143155. Rose, G. (1985). Sick individuals and sick populations. International Journal of Epidemiology, 14(1), 3238. Rose, G. A. (1992). The strategy of preventative medicine. Oxford: Oxford University Press. ARTICLE IN PRESS 1670 T.A. Glass, M.J. McAtee / Social Science & Medicine 62 (2006) 16501671 Rose, G. A., & Day, S. (1990). The population mean predicts the number of deviant individuals. British Medical Journal, 301, 10311034. Rosmond, R., & Bjorntorp, P. (2000). Occupational status, cortisol secretory pattern, and visceral obesity in middle-aged men. Obesity Research, 8(6), 445450. Ross, C. E., & Mirowsky, J. (2001). Neighborhood disadvantage, disorder, and health. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 42(3), 258276. Ross, C. E., & Wu, C. L. (1996). Education, age, and the cumulative advantage in health. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 37(1), 104120. Rothman, K. J. (1986). Modern epidemiology. Boston: Little, Brown and Company. Rothman, K. J. (1988). Causal inference. Chestnut Hill, MA: Epidemiology Resources Inc. Sampson, R. J., & Morenoff, J. D. (1997). Ecological perspectives on the neighborhood context of urban poverty: Past and present. In J. Brooks-Dunn, G. J. Duncan, & J. L. Aber (Eds.), Neighborhood poverty. Policy implications in studying neighborhoods, Vol. 2 (pp. 122). New York: Russell Sage. Sampson, R. J., Morenoff, J. D., & Gannon Rowley, T. (2002). Assessing neighborhood effects: Social processes and new directions in research. Annual Review of Sociology, 28, 443478. Sampson, R. J., Raudenbush, S. W., & Earls, F. (1997). Neighborhoods and violent crime: A multilevel study of collective efcacy. Science, 277(5328), 918924. Savitz, D. A. (1994). In defense of black box epidemiology. Epidemiology, 5(5), 550552. Schnall, P. L., Landsbergis, P. A., & Baker, D. (1994). Job strain and cardiovascular disease. Annual Review of Public Health, 15, 381411. Schwartz, S., & Diez-Roux, A. V. (2001). Commentary: Causes of incidence and causes of casesa Durkheimian perspective on Rose. International Journal of Epidemiology, 435439. Seeman, T. E., McEwen, B. S., Rowe, J. W., & Singer, B. H. (2001). Allostatic load as a marker of cumulative biological risk: MacArthur studies of successful aging. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 98(8), 47704775. Sheps, D. S., Freedland, K. E., Golden, R. N., & McMahon, R. P. (2003). ENRICHD and SADHART: Implications for future biobehavioral intervention efforts. Psychosomatic Medicine, 65(1), 12. Shonkoff, J. P., & Phillips, D. A. (Eds.). (2000). From neurons to neighborhoods: The science of early childhood development. Washington, DC: National Academy Press. Shy, C. M. (1997). The failure of academic epidemiology: Witness for the prosecution. American Journal of Epidemiology, 145(6), 479484 (discussion 485477). Siegrist, J. (1995). Emotions and health in occupational life: New scientic ndings and policy implications. Patient Education and Counseling, 25(3), 227236. Siegrist, J., Klein, D., & Voigt, K. H. (1997). Linking sociological with physiological data: The model of effort-reward imbalance at work. Acta Physiologica Scandinavica. Acta Physiologica Scandinavica Supplementum, 640, 112116. Siegrist, J., & Marmot, M. (2004). Health inequalities and the psychosocial environment-two scientic challenges. Social Science & Medicine, 58(8), 14631473. Siegrist, J., Peter, R., Cremer, P., & Seidel, D. (1997). Chronic work stress is associated with atherogenic lipids and elevated brinogen in middle-aged men. Journal of Internal Medicine, 242(2), 149156. Singh-Manoux, A., Ferrie, J. E., Chandola, T., & Marmot, M. (2004). Socioeconomic trajectories across the life course and health outcomes in midlife: Evidence for the accumulation hypothesis? International Journal of Epidemiology, 33(5), 10721079. Smith, G. D. (2002). Commentary: Behind the Broad Street pump: Aetiology, epidemiology and prevention of cholera in mid-19th century Britain. International Journal of Epidemiology, 31(5), 920932. Spiegel, K., Leproult, R., LHermite-Baleriaux, M., Copinschi, G., Penev, P. D., & Van Cauter, E. (2004). Leptin levels are dependent on sleep duration: Relationships with sympathovagal balance, carbohydrate regulation, cortisol, and thyrotropin. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism, 89(11), 57625771. Stallones, R. A. (1980). To advance epidemiology. Annual Review of Public Health, 1, 6982. Stallones, R. A. (1983). Mortality and the multiple risk factor intervention trial. American Journal of Epidemiology, 117(6), 647650. Stehbens, W. E. (1985). The concept of cause in disease. Journal of Chronic Diseases, 38(11), 947950. Stunkard, A. J. (1977). Obesity and the social environment: Current status, future prospects. Annual New York Academy of Sciences, 300, 298320. Susser, M. (1973). Causal thinking in the health sciences: Concepts and strategies of epidemiology. New York: Oxford University Press. Susser, M. (1995). The tribulations of trialsIntervention in communities. American Journal of Public Health, 85(2), 156158. Susser, M. (1998). Does risk factor epidemiology put epidemiology at risk? Peering into the future. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health, 52(10), 608611. Susser, M., & Susser, E. (1996). Choosing a future for epidemiology: II. From black box to Chinese boxes and eco-epidemiology. American Journal of Public Health, 86(5), 674677. Swinburn, B., Egger, G., & Raza, F. (1999). Dissecting obesogenic environments: The development and application of a framework for identifying and prioritizing environmental interventions for obesity. Preventive Medicine, 29(6 Pt 1), 563570. Syme, S. L. (1987). Social determinants of disease. Annals of Clinical Research, 19(2), 4452. Tataranni, P. A., Larson, D. E., Snitker, S., Young, J. B., Flatt, J. P., & Ravussin, E. (1996). Effects of glucocorticoids on energy metabolism and food intake in humans. American Journal of Physiology, 271(2 Pt 1), E317E325. Taylor, R. B. (1997). Social order and disorder of street blocks and neighborhoods: Ecology, microecology, and the systemic model of social disorganization. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency, 34(1), 113155. Taylor, S. E., Repetti, R. L., & Seeman, T. (1997). Health psychology: What is an unhealthy environment and how does it get under the skin? Annual Review of Psychology, 48, 411447. Teret, S. P., & Wintemute, G. J. (1993). Policies to prevent rearm injuries. Health Aff (Millwood), 12(4), 96108. Terris, M. (1980). Epidemiology as a guide to health policy. Annual Review of Public Health, 1, 323344. ARTICLE IN PRESS T.A. Glass, M.J. McAtee / Social Science & Medicine 62 (2006) 16501671 The International Rescue Committee. (2005). Mortality in the democratic republic of Congo: Results from a nationwide survey. Kinshasa, Nairobi, Brussels: IRC. Uchino, B. N., Cacioppo, J. T., & Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K. (1996). The relationship between social support and physiological processes: A review with emphasis on underlying mechanisms and implications for health. Psychological Bulletin, 119(3), 488531. Van de Mheen, H. D., Stronks, K., & Mackenbach, J. P. (1998). A lifecourse perspective on socio-economic inequalities in health: The inuence of childhood socio-economic conditions and selection processes. In M. Bartley, D. Blane, & G. D. Smith (Eds.), The sociology of health inequalities. Oxford, UK: Blackwell. van Lenthe, F. J., & Mackenbach, J. P. (2002). Neighbourhood deprivation and overweight: The GLOBE study. International Journal of Obesity and Related Metabolic Disorders, 26(2), 234240. van Rossum, C. T., Shipley, M. J., van de Mheen, H., Grobbee, D. E., & Marmot, M. G. (2000). Employment grade differences in cause specic mortality. A 25 year follow up of civil servants from the rst Whitehall study. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health, 54(3), 178184. 1671 Vandenbroucke, J. P. (1988). Is the causes of cancer a miasma theory for the end of the twentieth century? International Journal of Epidemiology, 17(4), 708709. Wamala, S. P., Wolk, A., & Orth-Gomer, K. (1997). Determinants of obesity in relation to socioeconomic status among middle-aged Swedish women. Preventive Medicine, 26(5 Pt 1), 734744. Wildsmith, E. M. (2002). Testing the weathering hypothesis among Mexican-origin women. Ethnicity and Disease, 12(4), 470479. Williams, D. R. (1996). Racism and health: A research agenda. Ethnicity and Disease, 6(12), 18. Wilson, E. O. (1998). Consilience: The unity of knowledge. New York: Alfred A. Knopf, Inc. Wintemute, G. J. (1988). Handgun availability and rearm mortality. The Lancet, 2(8620), 11361137. Wintemute, G. J. (1999). The future of rearm violence prevention: Building on success. Journal of American Medical Association, 282(5), 475478. Woodward, M., Shewry, M., Smith, W. C., & Tunstall Pedoe, H. (1990). Coronary heart disease and socioeconomic factors in Edinburgh and North Glasgow. The Statistician, 39, 319329.

Find millions of documents on Course Hero - Study Guides, Lecture Notes, Reference Materials, Practice Exams and more. Course Hero has millions of course specific materials providing students with the best way to expand their education.

Below is a small sample set of documents:

Montgomery College - BI - 200
CAMILLOGOLGIThe neuron doctrine - theory and factsNobel Lecture December 11, 1906It may seem strange that, since I have always been opposed to the neuron theory - although acknowledging that its starting-point is to be found in my own work - I have
Montgomery College - BI - 200
Microbiology and ImmunologyGRADUATE STUDENT SEMINAR SCHEDULE2009-2010Switch Seminars must be given by October 2 due to GPS admission deadlines.Seminars are scheduled for Thursday at 9:00 a.m. in room 507/509 of the Duff Medical Building. One letter gr
Montgomery College - BI - 200
1LECTURE NOTES ON HUMAN RESPIRATORY SYSTEM PHYSIOLOGY(Dr. GL ERDEMLI)CONTENTS1. MECHANICS OF BREATHING: 2. REGULATION AND CONTROL OF BREATHING: 3. VENTILATION 4. LUNG VOLUMES AND PULMONARY FUNCTION TESTS 5. DIFFUSION 6. PERFUSION 7. GAS TRANSPORT TO T
Montgomery College - BI - 200
Skin (Integumentary System)considered an organ or an organ system bodys largest organ organ of greatest surface area:15-20 sq ft. (1.5-2 m2 ) General Functions: 1. protection mechanical chemical bacterial UV desiccation 2. temperature homeostasis >temp s
Montgomery College - BI - 200
Teaching aWorldwide Medical SchoolPathology Course OnlineLectures and LabsJohn R. Minarcik, MDwww.medicalschoolpathology.comwww.gopathdx.comDISCLOSURES:1) No trees or copyright lawyers wereharmed or killed in the production ofthis multimedia pre
Montgomery College - BI - 200
REVISED CURRICULUMOFM.B.B.SCurriculum Development ProjectSponsored byMinistry of EducationIslamabadUNIVERSITY GRANTS COMMISSIONH-9, ISLAMABAD2002CURRICULUM DIVISION, UGCProf. Dr. Altaf Ali G. ShaikhMr. Muhammad YounusMalik Ghulam AbbasMiss G
Montgomery College - BI - 200
INFORMATION-SEEKING BEHAVIOR OF SCIENTISTS AND THEIRADAPTATION TO ELECTRONIC JOURNALSbyAmy C. GleesonA Masters paper submitted to the facultyof the School of Information and Library Scienceof the University of North Carolina at Chapel Hillin partia
Montgomery College - BI - 200
Montgomery College - BI - 200
EPITHELIUMDr. Paul W. L. KwanTufts University School of MedicineLearning Objectives1.2.3.4.5.Know the main characteristics of epithelium.Understand the main functions of epithelium.Be able to classify various epithelia based on their histology.
Montgomery College - BI - 200
Tufts OpenCourseWare Cellular Adaptations to Disease/Cell Injury and Death II 2008 Tufts University1.Basic Human Pathology Lecture #2 Cellular Adaptations to Dis.2.Reaction of Cells to InjuryOCW: Basic Human Pathology: Parts I and II (M. Kahn)Page
Montgomery College - BI - 200
AFM.qxd 11/24/04 9:55 PM Page iFifth EditionPLANTPATHOLOGYAFM.qxd 11/24/04 9:55 PM Page iiAFM.qxd 11/24/04 9:55 PM Page iiiFifth EditionPLANTPATHOLOGYGEORGE N. AGRIOSDepartment of Plant PathologyUniversity of FloridaAmsterdam Boston Heidelberg
Montgomery College - BI - 200
DEVELOPMENT THROUGHTHE LIFESPAN, 4/e 2007Laura E. Berk, Illinois State UniversityISBN: 0-205-49125-1Visit www.ablongman.com/replocator to contact your local Allyn & Bacon/Longman representative.The colors in this document arenot an accurate represe
Montgomery College - BI - 200
Andrew J. DuBrin, Fundamentals of Organizational Behavior, 4/e, Mason, OH:Thomson South-Western, 20071The Nature and Scope ofOrganizational BehaviorThe Healthy Workplace Awards recognize organizations for their commitment to programs and policies that
Montgomery College - BI - 200
1CENTRAL SERICULTURAL RESEARCH & TRAINING INSTITUTEBIOINFORMATICS CENTREDr. C. K. KambleDirector & CoordinatorCentral Silk Board (Ministry of Textiles Govt. of India)Srirampura, Manandavadi Road, Mysore 570 008Ph. 0821-2362547 Fax : 2362845E-mail
Montgomery College - BI - 200
The Encyclopedia ofApplied Animal Behaviourand WelfareThis page intentionally left blankThe Encyclopedia ofApplied Animal Behaviourand WelfareEditor-in-chiefDaniel S. MillsUniversity of LincolnDepartment of Biological SciencesRiseholme, Lincoln
Montgomery College - BI - 200
1.1Environment as Stress Factor: Stress Physiology of PlantsPlants are bound to places. They, therefore, have to beconsiderably more adaptable to stressful environments andmust acquire greater tolerance to multiple stresses thananimals and humans. Th
Montgomery College - BI - 200
Montgomery College - BI - 200
ScienceDirect College EditionSocial & Behavioral SciencesBooks CollectionSince the ScienceDirect College Edition launched in 2005, weve spoken tohundreds of college librarians. Weve learnt that attracting students with the mostpromise is a challenge
Montgomery College - BI - 200
Clark: Part2 Page 1Antibody-Antigen ReactionsMike Clark(adapted with permission from Geoff Hale's Lectures)Department of Pathology, University of CambridgeAffinity of antibody antigen reactionsThe binding of antibody to antigen is a reversible proce
Montgomery College - BI - 200
FIRE BEHAVIOR SCIENCE ADVANCEMENT PLANA PLAN FOR ADDRESSINGPHYSICAL FIRE PROCESSESWITHIN THE CORE FIRE SCIENCE PORTFOLIOEcosystemEcosystemIntegrityEnviron.QualityHumanWelfareFire RegimesClimate& Air QualityManaging Fire EffectsEconomicsAnd
Montgomery College - BI - 200
THE WRITINGS OF ANNA FREUDIn 8 Volumes, published byInternational Universities Press Inc:1. INTRODUCTION TO PSYCHOANALYSIS Lectures for Child Analysts and Teachers 1922-19352. THE EGO AND THE MECHANISMS OF DEFENCE (1936) 19663. INFANTS WITHOUT FAMIL
Montgomery College - BI - 200
An Actively Caring Model for Occupational Safety: A Field Test1.D. Steve Roberts, & E. Scott GellerAbstractActively caring refers to individuals caring enough about the health and safety of othersto act accordingly. Actively caring behavior in an indu
Montgomery College - BI - 200
THE MOTTER LECTURES ON SELECTED TOPICSIN SURGICAL PATHOLOGY.SERIES OFi8go-I.'_BY ROSWELL PARK, A.M., M.D.,OF BUFFALO,PROFESSOR OF SURGERY, MEDICAL DEPARTMENT, UNIVERSITY OF BUFFALO; SURGEON ro THE BUFFALO GENERAL HOSPITAL, ETC.LECTURE II.WOUND INF
Montgomery College - BI - 200
Course: Course: ANP 508 Histology of Farm Animals (3credits-Elective)Course Duration: Three hours per week for 15weeks (45hours) As taught in 2009/2010sessionLecturers:1 . Apata, D.F.PhD, M.Sc, BSc Animal Science (Ibadan).Department of Animal Produc
Montgomery College - BI - 200
Applied Sheep BehaviorPrepared by: Warren Gill, ProfessorAnimal Science DepartmentThe University of TennesseeIntroduction, The Basics and Sheep SensesSheep have evolved a unique and fascinating array of behavioral characteristicswhich have contribut
Montgomery College - BI - 200
Journal of Organizational BehaviorJ. Organiz. Behav. 29, 147154 (2008)Published online in Wiley InterScience(www.interscience.wiley.com) DOI: 10.1002/job.515EditorialPositive organizational behavior: Engagedemployees in ourishing organizationsARNOL
Montgomery College - BI - 200
Achieving Behaviour Change ForDengue Control: Methods, Scaling-Up,And SustainabilityWorking paper for the Scientific Working Group on Dengue Research, convened by the Special Programmefor Research and Training in Tropical Disease s, Geneva, 1-5 Octobe
Montgomery College - BI - 200
MEDICAL SERVICE CORPSBehavioral Science FieldAPPLICATION CHECKLIST(As of March 2009)JOIN THE NATIONS OLDEST MEDICAL ORGANIZATION. As Medical ServiceOfficer in the Behavioral Science field, you'll acquire skills that will make you aleader in your fie
Montgomery College - BI - 200
September 28, 2011How common is bad behavior in science?Kenneth D. Pimple, Ph.D.1This is a summary of recent empirical research on the rate of research misconduct and otherunacceptable behaviors in science.A. Survey of several thousand NIH-funded res
Montgomery College - BI - 200
ASSOCIATE OF ARTSBEHAVIORAL SCIENCE2011-2012GENERAL EDUCATIONSKILLS COMPONENTENGL 101 & 102ENGL 109COMM 101, 202, 203, 204, 205MTHPT 130, 137, 138REQ COMP NEED633MATH 123, 130, 130A & 130B, 137,143 & 144, 147, 157 & 257, 170, 254ID 123 Math
Montgomery College - BI - 200
CARRI Research Report 10BEHAVIORAL SCIENCE PERSPECTIVES ON RESILIENCEFran H. NorrisDartmouth Medical SchoolNational Center for PTSDNational Center for Disaster Mental Health ResearchNational Consortium for the Study of Terrorism and Responses to Ter
Montgomery College - BI - 200
Bachelor of ArtsReligious Education MajorBehavioral Science MinorMajor CoursesCourseBehavior Science MinorHoursCourseHoursCOM 3303 Communications for ChurchWorkers OR COM 3304 Group DynamicsREDU 1111 Spiritual Disciplines IPSY 1311 General Psy
Montgomery College - BI - 200
Boston College Law SchoolDigital Commons @ Boston College Law SchoolBoston College Law School Faculty Papers4-1-2005Behavioral Science Evidence in the Age of Daubert:Reflections of a SkepticMark S. BrodinBoston College Law School, brodin@bc.eduDig
Montgomery College - BI - 200
Behavioral Science HandbookPsych Faculty and ResourcesRalph Armstrong, MD: Consulting psychiatrist. Part of OPS rotation. Seespatients referred by residents and faculty with resident in the AFMC Fridaymornings. Adult psychiatry. Special interest in ad
Montgomery College - BI - 200
Virginia Department of Health ProfessionsHealthcare Workforce Data CenterBehavioral Science Workforce Advisory CommitteeDavid Boehm, L.C.S.W.WardenMarion Correctional CenterJohnston Brendel, LPC, LMFTClinical Associate ProfessorThe College of Will
Montgomery College - BI - 200
Political Psychology, Vol. 31, No. 4, 2010doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9221.2010.00768.xThe Behavioral Logic of Collective Action: PartisansCooperate and Punish More Than Nonpartisanspops_768595.616Oleg SmirnovStony Brook UniversityChristopher T. DawesUni
Montgomery College - BI - 200
Committed to excellence in Global EducationCOLLEGE OF BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES(DEPARTMENTS OF HUMAN BEHAVIOR AND PSYCHOLOGY)Mission StatementThe Faculty of the College of Behavioral Sciences is committed to the academic and experiential assistance andprep
Montgomery College - BI - 200
Behavioural FinanceMartin SewellUniversity of CambridgeFebruary 2007 (revised April 2010)AbstractAn introduction to behavioural nance, including a review of the majorworks and a summary of important heuristics.1IntroductionBehavioural nance is th
Montgomery College - BI - 200
The Journal of AppliedBehavioral Sciencehttp:/jab.sagepub.comUnlocking the Benefits of Diversity: All-Inclusive Multiculturalism andPositive Organizational ChangeFlannery G. Stevens, Victoria C. Plaut and Jeffrey Sanchez-BurksJournal of Applied Beha
Montgomery College - BI - 200
BIBLIOGRAPHY OF BEHAVIORAL SCIENCE RESEARCH IN UNINTENTIONAL INJURY PREVENTIONCompiled and Edited byDavid A. Sleet, PhD Krista Hopkins, MPHContributing EditorHelen Harber Singer, MPHCenters for Disease Control and PreventionNational Center for Injur
Montgomery College - BI - 200
AN ANNOTATED BIBLIOGRAPHY OF FOUNDATIONAL LITERATUREIN ORGANIZATIONAL BEHAVIOR AND DEVELOPMENTCompiled with commentary byPeter B. VaillUniversity of St. ThomasMinneapolis, MN 554032001Copyright, Peter B. Vaill, 2001.1Compiler's PrefaceThis bibli
Montgomery College - BI - 200
1ST. VINCENTS COLLEGESpring 2009BIO 212 Online Course4 creditsInstructor:Email:Phone:Class Website:Susan R. Capasso Ed.D., CGCscapasso@stvincentscollege.edu or Angel203-576-5323http:/stv.ctdlc.orgCourse Description: A continuation of BIO 211.
Montgomery College - BI - 200
Biology 47 - Hu man AnatomyLecture 2HistologyHIS TOL OG Y:histio =; logos = Tissue: Collaborative group of specialized cells : 4 Primary Tissues:1. Epithelial Ti ssue: Protective :2. Connecti ve Tissue: Specialized fluid connective tissues:3.
Montgomery College - BI - 200
Nervous System:Brain and Cranial Nerves(Chapter 14)The Brain and Cranial NervesSpinal cord ! reex = immediate responseBrain ! thinking = adaptable but slowerLecture MaterialsforAmy Warenda Czura, Ph.D.Suffolk County Community CollegeEastern Camp
Montgomery College - BI - 200
HUMAN ANATOMY - Biology 212Lecturer - Sandra GarverLecture Hall - AL-201Lecture Time - M/W 1:00pm - 1:50pm (sections 1-14)Office:Office Hrs:LS-221Ph o n e # : 6 1 9 - 5 9 4 - 8 0 2 7M 2 :00pm - 3:00pme-mail: sgarver@ sunstroke.sdsu.eduTues 11 :0
Montgomery College - BI - 200
Adaptive Immunity:Specic Defenses of the Host(Chapter 17)Immunity = resistance: defense againstforeign materialInnate immunity = defenses that are alwayspresent to provide instant protectionagainst infectionAdaptive immunity = induced, adapts to a
Montgomery College - BI - 200
BIOLOGY 485PRINCI PLES OF CELLULAR AND MOLECULAR IMMUNOLOGYCOURSE SYLLABUSSan Diego St ate Univ ersityFall 2010Monday and Wednes day 12:00 1:15 PMRoom LS 111Prof essor: Constantine D. Ts ouk as, Ph. D.Office: LS 406Telephone: 594-5764Office hour
Montgomery College - BI - 200
Biology 223 - HUMAN ANATOMY & PHYSIOLOGY ILECTURE COURSE OBJECTIVES SALADIN TEXT (Note there is considerable overlapwith the material in the lab objectives)CHAPTER 1 and ATLAS A: MAJOR THEMES OF ANATOMY & PHYSIOLOGY:ORIENTATION1. Define anatomy and p
Montgomery College - BI - 200
BOB BECK LECTURETAKE BACK YOUR POWER!CONQUERING INCURABLES WITH MICROCURRENTS!BLOOD ELECTRIFICATION is a proven, startling, rapid,inexpensive and safe discovery for proven remissions.A suppressed medical breakthrough now apparentlyguarantees anyone
Montgomery College - BI - 200
Dermatology Lecture List1. Introduction2. Eczematous Dermatitis & Urticaria3. Common Dermatologic Problems4. Path of Inflammatory Skin Disease5. Pathophysioogy of Non-Melanoma Skin Cancer6. Blistering Disorders7. Nevi and Malignant Melanoma01: Int
Montgomery College - BI - 200
GI Block I: Lecture List01) Esophageal Disorders . Nompleggi02) Gastric Mucosal Injury. Houghton03) Pathology of GI Inflammatory Disorders . Banner04) Pathology of Upper GI Tumors . Ayata05) Pathophysiology of Diarrhea & Malabsorption . Bhattacharya
Montgomery College - BI - 200
Bachelor of Physiotherapy - BPT(4 Year Degree Course)REGULATION & CURRICULUM2006Rajiv Gandhi University of Health Sciences, Karnataka4th T Block, Jayanagar, Bangalore 560 041.1Revised Ordinance Governing Regulations and Curriculum ofBPT 4 Year Cou
Montgomery College - BI - 200
"A New, Semi-Automated Method ofGrading Invasive and Non-InvasiveBreast Carcinoma Using ImageAnalysisOssama Tawfik, MD, PhDProfessor-Vice ChairmanDirector of Anatomic and Surgical PathologyKansas University Medical CenterAlexandria, Egypt July 1-
Montgomery College - BI - 200
Montgomery College - BI - 200
24 MARCH 1979BRITISH MEDICAL JOURNAL777those hospitals in Great Britain where endoscopy is freelyavailable. The procedure often facilitates an earlier decisionon surgery and enables unnecessary laparotomy to be avoided.There can be few diagnostic te
Montgomery College - BI - 200
BRITISHBEINGTHEMEDICALJOURNALOFTHEBRITISHJOURNAL:MEDICALASSOCIATION.LONDON: SATURDAY, JULY 3, 1869.LECTURESONTHE HISTOLOGY OF THE EYE:(BEING THE ARRIS AND GALE ANATOMICAL LECTURES.)Delivered at the Royal College of Surgeons of England, 7un
Montgomery College - BI - 200
Behavioral Research Methods and StatisticsMAS class, Spring 2010MAS 964Class time:12:00-3:00, Thursdays6 week class beginning Feb 4th ending Apr 1stLocation:E14-525Credits:3-0-3, G-levelInstructors:Matthew Goodwin, Ph.D.Office Hours: TBDmgood
Montgomery College - BI - 200
The Impact of Modern GeneticsD ONALD D. B ROWNT HE T ANNER L ECTURES O N H UMAN V ALUESDelivered atBrasenose College, Oxford UniversityMay 1 5 and 16, 1984D ONALD D. BROWN was born in Cincinnati, Ohio, in1931, a nd educated at Dartmouth College and
Montgomery College - BI - 200
The Journal of Applied Behavioral Sciencehttp:/jab.sagepub.comNurturing Collaborative Relations: Building Trust in Interorganizational CollaborationSiv Vangen and Chris HuxhamJournal of Applied Behavioral Science 2003; 39; 5DOI: 10.1177/0021886303039
Montgomery College - BI - 200
F R A N K M . BU R N E TImmunological recognition of selfNobel Lecture, December 12, 1960I like to think that when Medawar and his colleagues showed that immunological tolerance could be produced experimentally the new immunologywas born. This is a sc
Montgomery College - BI - 200
What is Animal Behavior?Animal behavior is the scientific study of everything animals do, whether the animals aresingle-celled organisms, invertebrates, fish, amphibians, reptiles, birds, or mammals. It involvesinvestigating the relationship of animals