MacCounReuter_EvaluatingAlternativeCannabisRegimes

MacCounReuter_EvaluatingAlternativeCannabisRegimes - B R I...

Info icon This preview shows page 1. Sign up to view the full content.

View Full Document Right Arrow Icon
This is the end of the preview. Sign up to access the rest of the document.

Unformatted text preview: B R I T I S H J O U R N A L O F P S YC H I AT RY ( 2 0 0 1 ) , 1 7 8 , 1 2 3 ^ 1 2 8 Evaluating alternative cannabis regimes*{ ROBERT MacCOUN and PETER REUTER Background Cannabis policy Background continues to be controversial in North America,Europe and Australia. Aims To informthis debate, we examine alternative legal regimes for controlling cannabis availability and use. Method We review evidence on the effects of cannabis depenalisation in the USA, Australia and The Netherlands.We update and extend our previous (MacCoun & Reuter,1997) empirical comparison of cannabis prevalence statistics in the USA,The Netherlands and other European nations. Results The available evidence indicates that depenalisation of the possession of small quantities of cannabis does not increase cannabis prevalence.The Dutch experience suggests that commercial promotion and sales may significantly increase cannabis prevalence. Conclusions Alternatives to an aggressively enforced cannabis prohibition are feasible and merit serious consideration. A model of depenalised possession and personal cultivation has many of the advantages of outright legalisation with few of its risks. Declaration of interest Funded by the Alfred P. Sloan Foundation.The authors have no financial interest in the outcome of the research. *This paper is excerpted from MacCoun & Reuter (2001), with the permission of Cambridge University Press.The work was supported by the Alfred P. Sloan Foundation through a grantto RAND's Drug Policy Research Center. { See editorial, p. 98, this issue. Cannabis is the cutting-edge drug for reform, the only politically plausible candidate for major legal change, at least decriminalisation (removal of criminal penalties for possession) and perhaps even outright legalisation (permitting production and sale). Compared with other drugs, the harms, physiological or behavioural, are less severe and the drug is better integrated into the culture. Throughout Western Europe and in the Antipodes there is pressure for reductions in the punitiveness of the marijuana regime. This paper attempts to project the likely consequences of substantial changes in the basic legal regime for cannabis and to offer an assessment of those consequences. The best evidence on the effects of liberalising marijuana policy comes from The Netherlands which has experienced both decriminalisation and commercialisation, though without ever changing formal law. In this paper, we summarise and extend our analysis of the Dutch cannabis policy (MacCoun & Reuter, 1997). The Dutch experience, together with those of a few other countries with more modest policy changes, provides a moderately good empirical case that removal of criminal prohibitions on cannabis possession (decriminalisation) will not increase the prevalence of marijuana or any other illicit drug; the argument for decriminalisation is thus strong. Making cannabis fully legal is likely to increase its use substantially because of promotion, particularly in the USA with its peculiar dedication to commercial free speech; that is possibly undesirable. An intermediate model can be devised which may be preferable to either legalisation or simple decriminalisation. DUTCH CANNABIS POLICY In compliance with international treaty obligations, Dutch law states unequivocally that cannabis is illegal. Yet in 1976 the Dutch adopted a formal written policy of non-enforcement for violations involving possession or sale of up to 30 g of cannabis ± a sizeable quantity, since few users consume more than 10 g a month (probably 25±35 joints) (Cohen & Sas, 1998). In 1995, in response to domestic and international pressures, this threshold for possession was lowered to 5 g. Moreover, a formal written policy regulates the technically illicit sale of those small amounts in open commercial establishments; a 500 g limit on trade stocks was established in 1995. Enforcement against those supplying larger amounts is aggressive; in 1995 the Dutch government seized 332 tonnes of cannabis, about 44 per cent of the total for the European Union as a whole (Ministry of Foreign Affairs et al, 1995). Between 1976 and 1986, a set of guidelines emerged stipulating that coffee-shop owners could avoid prosecution by complying with five rules: (a) no advertising; (b) no hard drug sales on the premises; (c) no sales to minors; (d) no sales transactions exceeding the quantity threshold; and (e) no public disturbances (Ministry of Foreign Affairs et al, 1995). In 1980, Ministry of Justice guidelines decentralised implementation, providing greater local discretion. As a result, enforcement became more lenient in Dutch cities, and somewhat stricter in smaller towns (Jansen, 1991). The effect is illustrated graphically in Dutch geographer A. C. M. Jansen's (1991) maps plotting cannabis coffee-shop locations in Amsterdam. He depicts nine locations in 1980, 71 in 1985 and 102 by 1988. (A location may correspond to more than one coffee shop.) Jansen notes that ``the first coffee shops were usually situated in unattractive buildings in backstreets'' (p. 69), but observes that over the course of the 1980s the shops spread to more prominent and accessible locations in the central city; they also began to promote the drug more openly. The cumulative effect on these formal, quasi-formal and informal policies is to make cannabis readily available at minimal legal risk to interested Dutch adults. There are approximately 1200 coffee shops selling cannabis in The Netherlands (Abraham et al, 1999, p. 93). Most offer an international variety of marijuana and hash strains of varying potency levels. Gram prices are 5 to 25 guilders ($2.50 to $12.50) (Kraan, 1994) compared with 12 3 M a c C OUN & R E U T E R US figures of $1.50 to $15.00. It is possible that the Dutch marijuana is of higher potency and `quality', a much less welldefined term but one which experienced smokers use. No data are available in either dimension. The continued high price of marijuana in The Netherlands probably reflects the aggressive enforcement against large-scale growers and distributors. OUTCOMES In MacCoun & Reuter (1997), we considered three key policy questions. Are levels of cannabis use higher in The Netherlands than in other Western nations? Did levels of cannabis use in The Netherlands increase following the 1976 depenalisation and subsequent de facto legalisation? And has the policy change weakened the statistical association between marijuana and use of other drugs? Here, we briefly summarise and extend those findings. In the final section, we clarify our interpretation of those findings and possible implications for US cannabis policy. Prevalence of cannabis use inThe Netherlands, USA, Denmark and Germany At the very least, meaningful cross-sectional comparisons of drug use should be matched for survey year, measure of prevalence (lifetime use, past-year use, or past-month use), and age groups covered in the estimate. Failure to meet these criteria has led to grossly discordant comparisons in which, for example, rates among 12- to 17-yearolds in one country are compared with those among 18-year-olds in another, all being called adolescents or teenagers (e.g. Associated Press, 3 October 1997 (P. Recer; published under different titles in various US newspapers); Los Angeles Times, 26 July 1998 (R. Housman; letter)). Our 1997 paper presented 15 comparisons that met these criteria. Table 1 extends the list to 28: 16 comparisons to the USA, three to Denmark, two to West Germany, one to Sweden, one to Helsinki, one to France, and four to the UK. We identified 15 comparisons in 1997. Here we add 13 additional comparisons. Some of these pre-date that paper but were unknown to us at the time it was written. Two others (lifetime use among those 12 and older in Tilburg and Utrecht in 1995) were omitted from that study by an oversight. Including the 124 latter increases the amount by which the US rates exceed those in Utrecht (from a 0.3% difference to a 1.4% difference) and especially Tilburg (from a 3.4% difference to a 7.9% difference). This does not change our substantive conclusion that ``US rates are . . . similar to that of Utrecht, and higher than that of Tilburg'' (MacCoun & Reuter, 1997, p. 49). All but two occur in the 1990s, during the period we have characterised as de facto legalisation, not just depenalisation. Four contrasts compare national estimates from The Netherlands and the USA; three show negligible differences between the two countries (within sampling error), while the newest estimate (Abraham et al, 1999) suggests that US prevalence is much higher. This discrepant result may be attributable to the inclusion of older adults in the latter comparison, or due to some difference between the Centre for Drug Research (CEDRO) (household) and Trimbos-institut (school-based) national survey methodologies. Twelve comparisons involve US national data and a Dutch city. Six contrasts pair the USA with an estimate from Amsterdam ± a large urban setting with a visible drug culture. American surveys indicate little difference, on average, between large metropolitan samples and the USA as a whole (Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration, 1979± 1999), but the estimates in Table 1 suggest that Amsterdam has a higher fraction of marijuana users than smaller Dutch communities. US rates are basically identical to those in Amsterdam and Utrecht, and higher than those in Tilburg (Langemeijer, 1997; also see Abraham et al, 1999). Unfortunately, many of the available contrasts between The Netherlands and her European neighbours suffer from the same weakness, comparing rates for an entire nation as a whole to those in the largest city of another nation. On average, Dutch prevalence rates are about 5 percentage points higher than their European neighbours; a 7% difference if one excludes the extreme Copenhagen contrast; a 6% difference if one also excludes the extreme Sweden contrast. Additional evidence, presented below, suggests that in recent years The Netherlands has had higher rates than Oslo, Norway. On the other hand, much of this higher Dutch rate is attributable to comparisons limited to Amsterdam, and we have seen that Amsterdam has higher rates than Tilburg and Utrecht. On balance, we conclude that Dutch rates are somewhat lower than those of the USA but somewhat higher than those of some, but not all, of its neighbours. Amsterdam's level of marijuana use is marijuana comparable to that of the USA. Trends in the prevalence of cannabis use Did levels of cannabis use in The Netherlands increase following the 1976 depenalisation and subsequent de facto legalisation? We examined Dutch lifetime prevalence data from various sources between 1970 and 1996 (MacCoun & Reuter, 1997). (Past-month or past-year prevalence estimates would be more informative but are scarce, especially prior to 1986.) The data for ages 16±17 and 18±20 come from a periodic national school survey (de Zwart et al, 1997); the data for ages 16± 19 in Amsterdam come from a periodic city survey (Sandwijk et al, 1995); and the trend line for 1970±1983 reflects a synthesis of various early estimates based on a multivariate analysis by Driessen et al (1989). The trend line implies that, among Dutch adolescents, cannabis use was actually declining somewhat in the years prior to the 1976 change and that the change had little if any effect on levels of use during the first 7 years of the new regime. Unfortunately, we lack data on the stringency of enforcement in the years immediately prior to the change in law, though the trend lines are fairly smooth and declining for at least 6 years prior to 1976. In the 1984±1996 period, which we characterise as a progression from depenalisation to de facto legalisation, these surveys reveal that the lifetime prevalence of cannabis in Holland has increased consistently and sharply. For the age group 18±20, the increase is from 15% in 1984 to 44% in 1996; past-month prevalence for the same group rose from 8.5% to 18.5% (de Zwart et al, 1997). Is this an effect of the emergence of de facto legalisation? Two comparison series offer insight: the US Monitoring the Future annual survey of high-school seniors (Bachman et al, 1998), and an annual survey of Oslo youth, aged 15 to 21 (Norwegian Ministry of Health and Social Affairs, 1997). The USA and Norway both strictly forbid cannabis sales and possession, and E VA LUAT I N G A LT E R N AT I V E C A NN A B I S R E G I M E S IN Table 1 Comparing cannabis use inThe Netherlands and other nations Age group Year Prevalence type Dutch location Dutch prevalence (%) Contrast location Contrast prevalence (%) Difference (%) Netherlands1 v. USA2 12 to 17 1992 Lifetime Netherlands 12.6 USA 10.6 Approx. 18 1992 Lifetime Netherlands 34.5 USA 32.6 1.9 Approx. 18 1996 Lifetime Netherlands 44.0 USA 44.9 70.9 12 and older 1997 Lifetime Netherlands 15.6 USA 32.9 717.3 Mean Dutch^US difference: 2.0 73.6 Tilburg (population 165 000) v. USA 3 2 12 and older 1995 Past month Tilburg 2.4 USA 4.7 12 and older 1995 Past year Tilburg 4.0 USA 8.4 74.4 12 and older 1995 Lifetime Tilburg 13.9 USA 31.0 717.1 Mean Tilburg^US difference: 72.3 77.9 Utrecht3 (population 235 000) v. USA2 12 and older 1995 Past month Utrecht 4.3 USA 4.7 70.4 12 and older 1995 Past year Utrecht 8.2 USA 8.4 70.2 12 and older 1995 Lifetime Utrecht 27.4 USA 31.0 73.6 Mean Utrecht^US difference: 71.4 Amsterdam (population 700 000) v. USA 4 2 12 and older 1994 Past month Amsterdam 6.7 USA 4.8 12 and older 1994 Past year Amsterdam 10.5 USA 8.9 1.7 12 and older 1994 Lifetime Amsterdam 29.1 USA 32.6 73.5 35 and older 1994 Past month Amsterdam 3.5 USA 2.3 1.2 35 and older 1994 Past year Amsterdam 5.8 USA 4.1 1.7 35 and older 1994 Lifetime Amsterdam 22.0 USA 25.4 73.4 Mean Amsterdam^US difference: 2.0 70.1 Netherlands v. other European nations Approx. 18 1990 Lifetime Netherlands5 28.0 Copenhagen6 52.0 20 to 24 1994 Past year Amsterdam4 25.0 Denmark7 16.0 9.0 25 to 29 1994 Past year Amsterdam4 18.2 Denmark7 7.0 11.2 12 to 29 1990 Lifetime Amsterdam4 33.0 W. Germany6 16.0 17.0 25 to 29 1994 Past year Amsterdam 18.2 W. Germany 5.6 12.6 15 1996 Lifetime Netherlands8 29.0 Sweden9 7.2 21.8 15 1992 Lifetime Netherlands8 18.1 Helsinki6 10.0 8.1 16 to 59 1994 Past year Amsterdam4 13.7 UK9 13.0 0.7 16 to 39 1994 Past year Amsterdam4 17.8 UK9 8.0 9.8 29.3 UK11 41.0 711.8 4 7 724.0 15 to 16 1995^9 Lifetime Netherlands10 15 to 16 1995^9 Past month Netherlands10 15.3 UK11 24.0 78.8 Approx. 18 1994 Lifetime Netherlands12 39.3 France13 29.0 10.3 Mean Dutch^European difference: 4.7 Mean after excluding Copenhagen comparison: 7.3 Mean after excluding Copenhagen and Sweden comparisons: 5.8 1. de Zwart et al (1997), except 1997 data from Abraham et al (1999). 2. National Household Survey on Drug Abuse (Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration, 1979^1999). In 1994, two different questionnaires were used; we have averaged the two sets of estimates, which were quite similar. 3. Langemeijer (1997). 4. Sandwijk et al (1995). 5. 1990 figure interpolated from 1988 and 1992 estimates in de Zwart et al (1997). 6. Hartnoll (1994). 7. European Monitoring Centre for Drugs and Drug Addiction (1996). 8. Unpublished estimate provided to us by Wil de Zwart of theTrimbos Institute (August 1998). 9. European Monitoring Centre for Drugs and Drug Addiction (1997). 10. The Independent (30 November 1997), confirmed for us by Wil de Zwart of theTrimbos Institute (August 1998). 11. 1995 European School Survey Project on Alcohol and Other Drugs (Hibell et al, 1997). 12. 1994 figure interpolated from 1992 and 1996 estimates in de Zwart et al (1997). 13. Boekhout van Solinge (1997). 12 5 M a c C OUN & R E U T E R aggressively enforced that ban throughout the period. Note that because the Oslo survey has a broader age range, these estimates are more meaningful for comparing trends over time than absolute differences in prevalence in any given year. The two comparison series behave very differently from the Dutch series, and from each other until 1992. The US rates increase until 1979 and then fall steadily and substantially until 1992, while the Oslo figures increase sharply only until 1972, and then fluctuate around a flat trend until 1992. Interestingly, during the period 1992 to 1996, all three nations have seen similar large increases, as have Canada (e.g. Adlaf et al, 1995) and the UK (Table 1). This weakens the hypothesis that the Dutch increases from 1992 to 1996 are attributable to Dutch policies per se; the fact that comparable increases occurred in nations with such different legal risks highlights the important role of non-policy influences that are only poorly understood. Nevertheless, the increases in Dutch prevalence from 1984 to 1992 provide the strongest evidence that the Dutch regime might have increased cannabis use among the young. As is seen in Fig. 1, this was a period in which use levels were fairly flat in Oslo and declining in the USA. Available estimates also suggest flat or declining use during this period in Catalunya, Stockholm, Hamburg and Denmark (Hartnoll, 1994), Germany as a whole (Reuband, 1992), Canada (Adlaf et al, 1995) and Australia (Mugford, 1992). Thus, unlike the widespread post-1992 rises, the 1984± 1992 escalation seems (almost) uniquely Dutch. In only one other location was cannabis use clearly increasing during this period ± Helsinki, where lifetime prevalence doubled among 15-year-olds between 1988 (5%) and 1992 (10%) (see Hartnoll, 1994). Could the removal of criminal penalties for possession and small-scale sales require 8 years to have an effect? We hypothesise that the dramatic mid-1980s escalation in Dutch cannabis use is the consequence of the gradual progression from a passive depenalisation regime to the broader de facto legalisation which allowed for greater access and increasing levels of promotion, at least until 1995 when the policy was revised ± in short, the effect of a shift from a depenalisation era to a commercialisation era. We are not claiming that the increases circa 1984±1992 are solely attributable to coffee-shop commercialisation, nor that commercialisation is synonymous with coffee-shop transactions. Commercialisation also involves the heightened salience and glamorisation (in the youth-cultural sense) that results from widespread, highly visible promotion ± the veiled references to cannabis in shop signs and advertisements, but also the explicit depictions in countercultural media ads, postcards and posters. The gateway association Has the Dutch policy change influenced the statistical association between marijuana Fig. 1 Estimated lifetime prevalence of cannabis among 18-year-olds in the USA, The Netherlands, Amsterdam and Oslo. 12 6 and use of other drugs? Though American hawks argue that more lenient cannabis policies might lead to greater levels of hard-drug use, a central rationale for the 1976 Dutch legal change was the notion that separating the soft- and hard-drug markets might actually weaken any gateway effect (Ministry of Foreign Affairs et al, 1995). Dutch policy may have had some success in separating these markets. Most Dutch cannabis users obtain that drug through either coffee shops or friends; few buy from street dealers. According to the 216 experienced Amsterdam cannabis users interviewed by Cohen & Sas (1998), harddrug sales at coffee shops are quite rare; only four reported that cocaine could be purchased, and only one knew of heroin sales at a shop. Among past-year cannabis users aged 18 and older in The Netherlands as a whole, 48% cite coffee shops as their place of purchase; only 0.7% report purchases from strangers on the street (Abraham et al, 1999). Fewer than 2% of past-year cocaine users report buying cocaine at coffee shops. In Amsterdam, as in the USA, almost all hard-drug users have used canna...
View Full Document

{[ snackBarMessage ]}

What students are saying

  • Left Quote Icon

    As a current student on this bumpy collegiate pathway, I stumbled upon Course Hero, where I can find study resources for nearly all my courses, get online help from tutors 24/7, and even share my old projects, papers, and lecture notes with other students.

    Student Picture

    Kiran Temple University Fox School of Business ‘17, Course Hero Intern

  • Left Quote Icon

    I cannot even describe how much Course Hero helped me this summer. It’s truly become something I can always rely on and help me. In the end, I was not only able to survive summer classes, but I was able to thrive thanks to Course Hero.

    Student Picture

    Dana University of Pennsylvania ‘17, Course Hero Intern

  • Left Quote Icon

    The ability to access any university’s resources through Course Hero proved invaluable in my case. I was behind on Tulane coursework and actually used UCLA’s materials to help me move forward and get everything together on time.

    Student Picture

    Jill Tulane University ‘16, Course Hero Intern