episodic memory than older adults have, both for real and imagined events
(
Despres & others, 2017
;
Sandrini & others, 2016
;
Wang & Cabeza, 2017
). A
recent study found that episodic memory performance predicted which
individuals would develop dementia 10 years prior to the clinical diagnosis
of the disease (
Boraxbekk & others, 2015
).
Autobiographical memories are stored as episodic memories.
A robust
finding in autobiographical memory is called the
reminiscence bump,
in
which adults remember more events from the second and third decades of
their lives than from other decades (
Berntsen & Rubin, 2002
;
Rathbone,
O’Connor, & Moulin, 2017
;
Steiner & others, 2014
). The “bump” is found
more for positive than negative life events. One study revealed support for
the reminiscence bump and indicated that these memories were more
distinct and more important for identity development (
Demiray, Gulgoz, &
Bluck, 2009
).
Semantic memory
is a person’s knowledge about the world. It includes a
person’s fields of expertise (such as knowledge of chess, for a skilled chess
player); general academic knowledge of the sort learned in school (such as
knowledge of geometry); and “everyday knowledge” about meanings of
words, famous individuals, important places, and common things (such as
who Nelson Mandela and Mahatma Gandhi are). For the most part, episodic
memory declines more than semantic memory in older adults (
Kuo & others,
2014
;
Lustig & Lin, 2016
).
Although older adults often take longer to retrieve semantic information,
usually they can ultimately retrieve it. As shown in
Figure 12
, semantic
memory continues to increase through the fifties, showing little decline
