how hearers break a string of sounds up into words and find the grammaticalpatterns in that string, what constraints memory places on these processes, whyspeech errors happen and so on. In contrast, models of language knowledgeconcentrate on describing the knowledge system that underlies these processes.Models of language processing usually assume a particular model of languageknowledge as a starting point, and place an emphasis on experimental methods.The models we discuss in this book (cognitive and formal models) are modelsof language knowledge. However, because cognitive linguists adopt the usage-based thesis, the interactional and goal-directed nature of language use iscentral to the cognitive model.4.1.4 ContextThe context in which an utterance or usage event is situated is central to thecognitive explanation. This is particularly true for word meaning, which isproteanin nature. This means that word meaning is rather changeable. Whilewords bring with them a conventional meaning, the context in which a word isused has important effects on its meaning. Furthermore, ‘context’ can mean anumber of different things.One kind of context issententialorutterance context. This relates to theother elements in the string. Consider example (2), where we are focusing inparticular on the meaning of the prepositionin:(2)a.The kitten is in the box.b.The flower is in the vase.c.The crack is in the vase.COGNITIVE LINGUISTICS:AN INTRODUCTION112
These examples involve spatial scenes of slightly different kinds, whereinreflects a spatial relationship between the figure and the reference object. In(2a) the figure,the kitten, is enclosed by the reference object,the box, so thatthe spatial relationship is one of containment. However, in the other twoexamples,indoes not prompt for quite the same kind of meaning. In (2b)the flower is not enclosed by the vase, since it partly protrudes from it.Equally, in (2c)indoes not prompt for a relationship of containment, becausethe crack is on the exterior of the vase. As these examples illustrate, themeaning ofinis not fixed but is derived in part from the elements that sur-round it.A second kind of context relates not to the other elements in the utteranceitself but to the background knowledge against which the utterance is pro-duced and understood. Consider example (3):(3)It’s dark in here.If said by one caver to another in an underground cavern, this would bea factual statement relating to the absence of light in the cavern. If uttered bya linguistics professor to a student who happened to be sitting next to the lightswitch in a poorly lit seminar room, this might be a request to turn the light on.If uttered by one friend to another upon entering a brilliantly lit room, it mightbe an ironic statement uttered for the purpose of amusement. As this range ofpossible meanings demonstrates, the context of use interacts with the speaker’sintentions and plays a crucial role in how this utterance is interpreted by the
Upload your study docs or become a
Course Hero member to access this document
Upload your study docs or become a
Course Hero member to access this document
End of preview. Want to read all 857 pages?
Upload your study docs or become a
Course Hero member to access this document
Term
Fall
Professor
smith john
Tags
Cognitive linguistics